Dual thinking for scientists
Marten Scheffer,
Wageningen Agricultural University, The NetherlandsJordi Bascompte,
Integrative Ecology Group, Estación Biológica de Doñana, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas, Sevilla, SpainTone K. Bjordam,
Studenterhytta i Nordmarka, Sørkedalen, Oslo, NorwayStephen R. Carpenter,
Center for Limnology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, USALaurie B. Clarke,
School of Education, University of Wisconsin, Madison, USACarl Folke,
Beijer International Institute of Ecological Economics, Royal Swedish Academy of Sciences, Stockholm, Sweden; Stockholm Resilience Centre, Stockholm University, SwedenPablo Marquet,
Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Avda. Libertador Bernardo OHiggins 340, Santiago, ChileNestor Mazzeo,
South American Institute for Resilience and Sustainability Studies, SARAS, Maldonado, Uruguay; CURE, Universidad de la República, Maldonado, UruguayMariana Meerhoff,
CURE,Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de la República, Maldonado, Uruguay; South American Institute for Resilience and Sustainability Studies, SARAS, Maldonado, UruguayOsvaldo Sala,
School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, USAFrances R. Westley,
University of Waterloo, School of Environment Enterprise and Development (SEED), Environment 3 (EV3), Waterloo, Canada
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5751/ES-07434-200203
Full Text: HTML 
Download Citation
Abstract
Recent studies provide compelling evidence for the idea that creative thinking draws upon two kinds of processes linked to distinct physiological features, and stimulated under different conditions. In short, the fast system-I produces intuition whereas the slow and deliberate system-II produces reasoning. System-I can help see novel solutions and associations instantaneously, but is prone to error. System-II has other biases, but can help checking and modifying the system-I results. Although thinking is the core business of science, the accepted ways of doing our work focus almost entirely on facilitating system-II. We discuss the role of system-I thinking in past scientific breakthroughs, and argue that scientific progress may be catalyzed by creating conditions for such associative intuitive thinking in our academic lives and in education. Unstructured socializing time, education for daring exploration, and cooperation with the arts are among the potential elements. Because such activities may be looked upon as procrastination rather than work, deliberate effort is needed to counteract our systematic bias.
Copyright © 2015 by the author(s). Published here under license by The Resilience Alliance. This article is under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License. You may share and adapt the work for noncommercial purposes provided the original author and source are credited, you indicate whether any changes were made, and you include a link to the license.