The following is the established format for referencing this article:
BurnSilver, S. B., J. M. Coleman, and J. Magdanz. 2022. Equality and equity in Arctic communities: how household-level social relations support community-level social resilience. Ecology and Society 27(3):31.ABSTRACT
Social and economic inequality are increasingly linked with greater vulnerability and compromised resilience for communities navigating ecological and institutional change. We focused on social resilience; i.e., the ability of foundational social institutions of sharing and cooperation in three Arctic Indigenous communities to maintain key social processes and structures in response to contemporary challenges. We explored two propositions: first, sharing and cooperation are distributional processes that increase the equality of access to wild foods at the community level. Second, sharing and cooperation embody cultural mechanisms that express trust and build social cohesion. Our analyses were based on household-level harvest and social network data that represented social ties and magnitudes of wild foods flowing from crews and between households. Qualitative and quantitative results indicated that material, emotional, and cultural outcomes of sharing and cooperation act across social levels—households to communities—to increase equality and equity. For all three communities, Lorenz curves and Gini coefficients indicated that distributions of wild food were more equal when sharing, cooperative-provisioning, and self-provisioning were considered than household self-provisioning alone. Network regressions emphasized close kinship and total harvest as social mechanisms strongly predictive of sharing outflows across communities (i.e., people share with family, and the more you have, the more you give). Income effects were mixed. There was evidence of different forms of need-based sharing in all communities, which suggests that social relationships also act as mechanisms to improve equity. Qualitative results linked decisions to share and cooperate with outcomes of well-being, and cultural integrity at household and community levels. While production of wild foods occurs at greater-than-household scales, the State manages wild food production at individual and household scales, which sets up conflicts between Indigenous communities and the State. Sharing and cooperative networks embedded in Arctic mixed economies are culturally derived and place-based institutions. Redistribution of resources through these networks, and the maintenance of social relationships to activate networks in times of need, increase the equality of outcomes—and therefore social resilience—at the community level in the face of rapid change.INTRODUCTION
A substantial body of literature links social inequality in social-ecological systems to increased vulnerability and compromised resilience (Adger 2000, Adger et al. 2005, Thomas and Twyman 2005). Leach et al. (2018) highlighted differences between equality (equal access to resources) and equity (distribution of resources based on the needs of recipients). Donkersloot et al. (2020) explored issues of equality, equity, and sustainability in Alaska’s salmon fisheries, noting that the Alaska Constitution mandates equality—not equity—in the allocation of Alaska’s renewable resources, and arguing that resource policies based on equality are “often at odds with the principle of equity.”
We explore equality and equity as valued principles in the culturally embedded sharing and cooperative practices of two Iñupiaq communities and one Gwich’in community in Alaska, focusing on implications for household- and community-level social resilience. Our approach acknowledges the Arctic as a landscape of resilience and immense environmental variability, one in which diverse human communities have developed cultural and social adaptations organized around principles of redistribution through sharing and cooperation. We use quantitative social network methods to describe the activation of these principles through natural resource redistributions—“wild food flows”—among households and crews. We examine the extent to which resource redistributions reduce—though not eliminate—inevitable inequalities in household-level wild food production. We also explore how local residents themselves perceive the effects of redistribution through sharing on household and community well-being, a concept allied to resilience but with stronger local relevance.
The contribution of individual- and household-level social relations to higher levels of community equality and equity in the Arctic is hardly a surprise (Bodenhorn 1990, Collings et al. 1998, Bodenhorn 2000, Wenzel et al. 2000, Kishigami 2004, Natcher et al. 2012, Kishigami 2013, Collings et al. 2016). However, we investigate these contributions with a grounded approach to the characterization, quantification, and categorization of wild food flows for a large, culturally diverse sample of Indigenous Alaskans (Charmaz 2006). This approach allows analyses of redistribution structures, factors that influence redistribution, and—perhaps most important—the contributions that culturally embedded sharing and cooperative practices make to community equality and equity, and therefore social resilience.
Background and theoretical framing
The three Arctic communities we studied have strong, place-based, social-ecological relationships that link people with each other, and people to the land and animals that sustain them. The focus here is on social resilience, defined as “the way in which individuals, communities, and societies adapt, transform, and potentially become stronger when faced with environmental, social, economic, or political challenges” (Cuthill et al. 2008:146). For Adger (2000), the term is analogous to ecological resilience but focuses on the ability of social institutions—in our case, sharing and cooperative networks—to maintain key elements of social function and structure in response to exposures (Brown 2014, Berman et al. 2017). These institutions are theorized here as forms of social capital, which are embedded in networks of social relationships, and act to provide access to resources, build trust, and facilitate cooperation in communities for mutual benefit (Lin 1999, Putnam 2000, Woolcock and Narayan 2000). Community resilience is defined as the ability of co-located groups of people to respond to changes without losing the critical structures, processes, and dynamics that are the basis for livelihoods and well-being (Berkes and Ross 2013).
The Arctic today is experiencing rapid and profound change, not only climate change but also the continuing impacts of two centuries of exploration, colonial expansion, assimilation pressures, and economic disruptions. Nonetheless, Arctic Indigenous peoples have persisted and continue to advocate for ways of life based on core values and practices deeply embedded in their cultures (Wilson 2014, Gadamus and Raymond-Yakoubian 2015, Thorton and Mamontova 2016). A predicted wholesale transition from subsistence-based livelihoods to market-based wage employment has not materialized. Instead, we see widespread and persistent mixed subsistence–cash economies grounded in cultural values around human–human and human–animal sharing relationships (Langdon 1991, BurnSilver et al. 2016, Magdanz et al. 2016). In recent decades, Arctic peoples have negotiated new institutional and governance arrangements to improve political, land, and mineral sovereignty, and to protect cultural integrity (Mitchell 2001, Burwell 2005). Yet, as Arctic mixed economies become more diverse, people are continuously pushed and pulled to adjust livelihoods to respond to regional opportunities and challenges (Reedy-Maschner 2009, BurnSilver and Magdanz 2019).
Contemporary Arctic livelihoods—mixed subsistence–cash economies—are built on three fundamental and integrated pillars: (1) households harvesting wild foods, (2) households generating cash through salaries, wage work, or the making of art, and (3) households and groups redistributing food and non-food resources through social relationships (Langdon and Worl 1981, Wolfe and Walker 1987, BurnSilver and Magdanz 2019). Scholars have described wild food sharing patterns in contemporary Arctic mixed economies for the Copper, Netsilik, and Iglulik Inuit of Canada and Greenland (Damas 1972, Hovelsrud-Broda 1999, 2000, Wenzel 2000, Harder and Wenzel 2012), for the Yupik of St. Lawrence Island, Alaska (Jolles and Oozeva 2002), for the Iñupiat of Alaska’s Arctic coast (Bodenhorn 2000, SRB&A 2018, Erickson 2020), and for Yup’ik of southwest Alaska (Langdon 1991). Sharing and cooperation create and maintain social relationships over time, with positive effects on the nutritional, social, emotional, and economic security of the larger group as high-harvesting households provision others (Smith and Wright 1989, Usher et al. 2003). Household-level actions (e.g., harvesting, working, sharing) scale up to community-level outcomes (BurnSilver et al. 2017). Viewed in this way, sharing and cooperative relationships are a set of social structures and processes that (1) materially affect equality of access to wild foods and other resources, and (2) contribute to well-being and social cohesion within communities and beyond. Outcomes of sharing and cooperation—material, emotional, and cultural—act across social levels—individuals to households to communities—to affect social resilience.
Early scholarship by explorers created an image of Yupik, Central Yup’ik, and Iñupiaq societies as egalitarian and non-hierarchical. However, ethnohistorian Burch (1980) presented a more nuanced view of Indigenous life in northwest Alaska before 1850. According to Burch (1980), people lived in semi-migratory groups made up of one or a few extended “local” families in which a productive hunting household or a productive individual—an umialik—held influence over and responsibility for the well-being of others, most of whom were kin (Burch 1975). In the North Slope dialect of the Iñupiaq language, umialik (umialgit) means “boat captain(s),” but also “boss,” “rich man,” or “church elder” (Webster and Zibell 1970, Seiler 2012). In Central Yup’ik, the equivalent terms are alularta or angyalek (Jacobson 2012). In Yupigestun (spoken on St. Lawrence Island), the equivalent terms are angyalek, angyalegtaq, and angyallighta. These terms reflect the unique roles and relationships linked to the bowhead whaling complex in the Alaskan and Russian Arctic, and imply that perfect equality was not the norm historically in Arctic societies. Patterns of sharing and cooperation were attenuated by interactions between climate and food availability. When food was plentiful, it was assumed that households could provide for themselves, and so household access to harvests was not equal. When food was scarce, it was shared from the umialik and his wife with those in need, and created greater equity among extended family groups (Burch 1980). But when famine threatened, all households were on equally bad footing, and groups dispersed.
Eleven Yupik and Iñupiaq communities in Alaska continue to hunt bowhead whales (Balaena mysticetus). In these communities, umialgit organize whaling crews that link individuals from multiple households together under the leadership of the umialgit and their spouses or partners. Preparing crews for the hunt requires provisioning by captains, their partners, crew members, and many other local and non-local households and entities; for example, local businesses or industry. When a crew is successful, it is a cause for celebration, but the sheer size of a bowhead whale creates a distribution challenge. To provide access to the harvest, successful umialgit distribute significant shares to crew members, other whaling crews, and households who initially provisioned them. Usually, these large crew or helper shares are then further divided and redistributed to others inside and outside communities (SRB&A 2018). Successive feasts and “household share” traditions also ensure that every individual and household in communities receives whale from successful crews (Alaska Eskimo Whaling Commission, personal communication). These general patterns hold true across whaling communities, but there are significant community-to-community differences grounded in cultural histories of place and environment.
Linked structural inequalities and distribution processes like those characterizing whaling complexes are an everyday feature of subsistence economies, regardless of the resources being harvested. Contemporary empirical evidence for these relationships comes from a program of comprehensive community harvest surveys initiated in rural Alaska around 1980, which quantified subsistence harvests at household and community levels. As data from this program accumulated, Alaska researchers found a pattern so consistent—about 30% of households in a given community accounted for about 70% of the community’s total wild harvest, by weight—that Wolfe (1987) termed it the “30:70 rule”. Wolfe et al. (2009) advanced several explanations for the skewed harvest distributions, including the differential capabilities of households, household developmental cycles, and household-level challenges. The credible assumption was that highly productive households—“super households”—were redistributing wild foods to households with lower harvests in the community, usually characterized simply as “sharing.” But the State program did not collect information about which households were sources or consumers (“sinks”) of the redistributed foods, nor did it attempt to quantify the amounts of wild foods being shared.
A methodological advance occurred in the mid-1990s when scholars began applying social network methods to subsistence food production in the Arctic. Initial work was limited to the identification of source and sink households and associated counts of ties (Magdanz et al. 2002, Dombrowski et al. 2013a, Ziker 2014, Natcher 2015, Collings et al. 2016, Ready and Power 2018, Ready 2018). Network approaches were also applied by subsistence researchers working in Indonesia (Nolin 2010), Fiji (Dacks et al. 2020), and elsewhere. These studies revealed similarities and differences in the structure of sharing networks. They highlighted a range of causes and motivations for sharing across large geographic and cultural distances, including kinship (Nolin 2011, Dombrowski et al. 2013b), prestige/leadership (i.e., costly signaling) (Ready and Power 2018), and reciprocal altruism (Nolin 2012).
Researchers subsequently expanded their protocols to ask community members to describe the quantities of wild foods they received. This documented the wild foods and other resources flowing from sources to sinks, a time-intensive data collection task infrequently attempted at community scales. Working with a sample of 35 Indigenous households in two adjacent communities in Nicaragua, Koster (2011) may have been the first to quantify subsistence food flows at a community scale. Working with a sample of 300 households in three widely dispersed communities in Arctic Alaska, “The Sharing Project” (Kofinas et al. 2016) documented wild foods flows into households across 20 social relations. The Sharing Project found that only about one-quarter of community-level wild food inflows by weight resulted from households’ own production for themselves, while three-quarters occurred through social relations (Kofinas et al. 2016). Project results were disseminated widely within communities and to regional collaborators. Ensuing comments by community partners emphasized that the method adhered strongly to points communities had been making about sharing and cooperation as integral to northern ways of life for decades. Data from The Sharing Project formed the basis of our analyses.
Two propositions
We considered two propositions: (1) sharing and cooperation are distributional processes that increase equality of access to wild foods at the community level, and (2) sharing and cooperation embody cultural mechanisms that express trust and build social cohesion. The first proposition emphasizes material benefits of sharing; the second highlights non-material benefits. Both propositions describe institutions that improve social resilience (Adger 2000).
Just as a household works together to provision its members with needed resources—children and elders have access to food and resources they would not have without more productive household members—sharing and cooperation among households and crews acts to increase equality of wild food access at the community level. Access to high-quality (i.e., nutrient- and protein-dense) wild foods has significant nutritional benefits for northern households (Johnson et al. 2009). To the extent that active hunters and their households have greater ability to distribute wild foods to those with less, equity within communities is also improved.
Community narratives about sharing commonly prioritize elders and single parents with children as those “in need”; i.e., elders who have done their part for the community over time, and particular families that require additional help (Okada 2010). Need might stem from low direct access to wild food due to age or choice, or lack of time, hunting ability, or equipment. For example, Collings et al. (2016) found that high-income households that are fully able to purchase store-bought food may still be perceived as “in need” of wild food if they hunt or fish only occasionally. The interaction between ability and need sets the stage for an exploration of the effects of sharing and cooperation on equality, equity, and social resilience in communities.
Subsistence goes beyond the means, methods, and outcomes of producing wild food: it includes enactment of cultural practices, values, and ways of knowing, while supporting spiritual and social relations (Active 1998, Raymond-Yakoubian and Angnaboogok 2017). Bodenhorn (1990:28) described sharing among the Iñupiat of Alaska’s North Slope as “a complex set of social actions, all of which create and maintain morally valued relationships that extend well beyond hunting itself.” Similarly, Alexie (2015:1) stated that Indigeneity and governance among her people, Teetł’it Gwich’in, “is premised on traditional principles of respect, sharing, and reciprocity.”
Sharing relationships also demonstrate care for others, including non-human persons. Such practices serve a fundamental social purpose in maintaining human connections and creating social cohesion, and in contributing to a sense of well-being, thriving, or happiness for individuals and communities (Kral et al. 2011). According to Adger (2000:351), it is this “cultural context of institutional adaptation…within different knowledge systems [that is] central to the resilience of institutions.” Capacity to deal with change is activated when people exercise agency (Brown and Westaway 2011, Berkes and Ross 2013) and when they perceive they have the self-efficacy to do so (Kruse et al. 2009). For Alaska’s Indigenous peoples, a foundational way their agency is expressed is through active engagement in hunting, fishing, gathering, and cooperative relationships. This suggests that social institutions like sharing and cooperation—which are of a place and of the people—are by their nature adaptive and critical for resilience.
In the following sections, we explore these propositions. Using measures of inequality, we examine the extent to which sharing and cooperative practices at the household and crew levels scale up to affect distributional equality of material food flows in communities and highlight the directionality of flows between households based on characteristics that may differentiate ability versus need. We do so based on livelihood, harvesting, and social network data that represent both existing ties and the magnitude of wild food flows between households within communities through social relationships. We use a network regression model to explore possible influences of household factors on the flows of wild foods among households.
We then integrate cultural narratives that describe sharing effects on household and community well-being. We use well-being as a proxy for social resilience because “resilience” is not a term in common use in the North, and outcomes associated with well-being overlap with social resilience constructs; e.g., social cohesion, trust, health, and connectedness (Duhaime et al. 2004, Healey Akearok et al. 2019).
THE SETTING
Alaska’s North Slope is a broad coastal plain stretching north and west from the foothills of the Brooks Range to the Beaufort and Chukchi Seas. It is home to the Iñupiat, a cultural subgroup of Iñuit circumpolar peoples who have access to a diverse array of marine, riverine, and terrestrial subsistence species. The Upper Yukon is a watershed that drains the southern slopes of the Brooks Range and flows ultimately into the Bering Sea. It is home to the Gwich’in, a subgroup of the Dene peoples, who are dependent primarily on the resources of interior rivers and boreal forests.
On the North Slope, Wainwright (Ulġuniq) and Kaktovik (Qaaktuġvik) are coastal Iñupiaq whaling communities (Fig. 1). In 2010, Wainwright had a total population of 556 people, of whom 92% were Alaska Native. Similarly, 90% of Kaktovik’s 239 residents were Alaska Native (US Census Bureau 2012). Wainwright and Kaktovik residents hunted, fished, and gathered 52 and 53 different species, respectively, during the study period.
Hunting of bowhead whales in particular is a critical part of being Iñupiaq. Both communities are represented on the Alaska Eskimo Whaling Commission, and the Indigenous right to whale for subsistence has been successfully negotiated and defended by the Alaska Eskimo Whaling Commission and US delegation to the International Whaling Commission since the late 1970s (Huntington 1989). Wainwright crews historically hunted the bowhead in spring in boats launched from the shorefast ice. Kaktovik crews hunt only in the fall in the open water. In 2009, Wainwright also began fall hunting due to increasingly uncertain spring ice conditions. Social relationships specific to whaling include those that coordinate hunting and harvesting labor through crew membership, and access to supplies and equipment (e.g., crew shares, towing shares, captains’ shares, helper shares), as well as processing and distribution of landed whales (e.g., household-to-household sharing and trade, helper shares, whaling captain’s feasts, and the annual Nalukataq whaling feasts).
Wainwright and Kaktovik are both part of the North Slope Borough and the Arctic Slope Regional Corporation (one of 13 regional corporations created by the Alaska Native Claims Settlement Act). Taxable oil infrastructure funds fire and health services in communities, as well as centralized water/sewer and natural gas systems. Wainwright and Kaktovik are accessible only by summer barge and daily bush air services.
On the northeast bank of the Chandalar River, 200 miles (322 km) south of Kaktovik, lies the Gwich’in Dene community of Venetie (Vááhtàáá). Cultural groups represented in this area include Neets’ai, Gwichyaa, and Dihaii Gwich’in (State of Alaska 2020). In 2010, Venetie’s population was 166, of whom 99% were Alaska Native. Venetie is not connected to Alaska’s road system but is accessible by bush aircraft and by small boat via the Chandalar River. Venetie homes are wood heated and unconnected to a central water system. Residents harvest, fish, and gather terrestrial and riverine species (39 species in 2009–2010).
Kaktovik and Venetie both depend on the Porcupine caribou herd (Rangifer tarandus granti): Kaktovik in spring and fall at the herd’s northern migration point on the coastal plain, and Venetie during the herd’s winter migration south. While lacking direct access to marine environments, Venetie residents receive bowhead whale, bearded seal (Erignathus barbatus), and beluga whale (Delphinapterus leucas) based on sharing and trading relationships with coastal residents (Kofinas et al. 2016).
Having opted out of the Alaska Native Claims Settlement Act in 1971, the two Native villages of Venetie and Arctic Village jointly own 1.8 million acres (728,434 ha) of their traditional lands in Alaska’s eastern interior, giving them a higher degree of territorial sovereignty than most other Indigenous Alaska communities but reducing their access to some Native corporation benefits under the Alaska Native Claims Settlement Act. Both Venetie and Arctic Village are members of the Council of Athabascan Tribal Governments, which provides health care and other services.
The study communities are similar in their dependence on wild foods but vary in the nature and extent of their agency over wild food access. Wainwright and Kaktovik depend primarily on marine mammals harvested in a marine commons managed by the federal government, and face limited direct hunting competition from others, even as the Beaufort and Chukchi Seas are increasingly congested with shipping, oil and gas operations, fishing, and other interests that are potentially in conflict with subsistence goals. In contrast, Venetie depends primarily on caribou, moose, and salmon harvested from tribal lands and waters where renewable wild resources are managed by the State of Alaska for the “common use” of all Alaska residents. The people of Venetie also face significant harvest competition from users beyond the boundaries of their lands, and expose themselves to legal jeopardy when they fail to conform to the State-mandated individual harvest reporting systems.
METHODS
Data collection
Data collection was conducted as part of the 3-year Sharing Project funded by the US Department of the Interior, Minerals Management Service (later Bureau of Ocean Energy Management). The project sought to understand the role of social relationships within mixed subsistence–cash economies in offsetting household vulnerability and building resilience to changes associated with potential offshore oil and gas development. Social relationships were a specific focus of the research, based on social capital (Putnam 2000) and network theories (Borgatti et al. 2009), which highlight that in addition to other household-level attributes, patterns of social ties between entities are predictive of social, health, and economic outcomes.
The Sharing Project was approved by the University of Alaska Fairbanks Institutional Review Board. All three communities formally consented to, actively participated in, and reviewed results of the research. All respondents were paid an hourly rate for their time, and interviews lasted an average of 90 minutes (minimum 30 min; maximum 3 h). Response rates were high: 146 households in Wainwright (96%), 70 in Kaktovik (82%), and 84 in Venetie (94%). For the analyses, non-local teacher households were removed, which resulted in samples of 133, 64, and 80 households for Wainwright, Kaktovik, and Venetie, respectively.
Fieldwork began in 2008–2009, when researchers conducted semi-directed interviews with key respondents (Bernard 2006) to identify key species and culturally resonant sharing and distribution patterns that were specific to each community. A process of open-coding, literature review, and iterative discussion with community members identified 20 unique social relationships through which 7–10 core fish and wildlife species flowed from households and crews into respondent households (see Kofinas et al. 2016:19-22). These relationships—including multiple types of shares resulting from cooperative harvest or processing efforts, and sharing resulting from household-to-household exchanges, among others—structured how network data were collected. In late 2010 and early 2011, researchers worked in teams (one academic researcher paired with one local interviewer) to carry out a household survey that collected demographic, employment, harvest, and social network data in Wainwright, Kaktovik, and Venetie. Using the 20 social relationships, the survey asked respondents to describe flows of food into their households (inflows) in local terms (e.g., a large cooler, half of one caribou, three ducks) during the preceding 12 months. All quantities were then converted to estimated pounds using the State’s standard wild food conversion factors (Alaska Department of Fish & Game 2020; see Appendix 1). Open-ended questions about reasons and processes for sharing were also asked during the Kaktovik and Wainwright surveys.
Many key sharing and cooperative relationships were captured in the study period covering 2009–2010, but not all. Individual meal sharing and flows of wild food into households from communal Thanksgiving and Christmas feasts were not documented. In Wainwright, captains and crew members from all three bowhead whales landed that year were interviewed. In Kaktovik, crew members from all successful crews were interviewed, but only one of three successful whaling captains was interviewed. This resulted in substantial under-documentation of crew contributions to crew whaling efforts in Kaktovik and umialik (captain’s) shares from those whales, as well as household-level social flows to households that were not interviewed (see Chapter 3 in Kofinas et al. 2016 for details on sampling methodology).
Quantitative data analyses
While the Sharing Project was fundamentally a social network project and network data were essential to our analyses, this analysis does not include network-specific statistics. We relied on Lorenz curves and Gini coefficients for estimates of the effect of social relations on equality, and on a network regression model for estimates of the influence of household attributes on flow patterns. For detailed descriptions of the network methods and for foundational network analyses and summary statistics, see Kofinas et al. (2016).
Conceptually, a social network consists of nodes and edges stored in an n x n socio-matrix, where each cell contains a value representing the presence/absence or, in a valued network, the strength of the relationships (edges) between each pair of nodes (i, j). In a directed network, the entry yij represents the value of the relationship from i to j, while the entry yji represents the value of the (reciprocal) relationship from j to i. In this study, network edges were both valued and directed. When, as here, multiple relations are collected, each relationship is stored in a separate matrix (e.g., moose sharing). Multiple matrices can be aggregated into a new matrix (e.g., all sharing). Networks often are represented graphically, as in Fig. 2, which illustrates the aggregation of all wild foods redistributed through social networks in Wainwright, Kaktovik, and Venetie. Sampling rates were high enough that most dyads (two households connected by an edge) were represented by two surveys, one from each household, and both reporting social and/or cooperative inflows from the other. Since only inflows were collected, flow data were not duplicated; outflows from one household were inflows to the other, thus providing a nearly complete set of network data.
A complex subsistence food distribution system can become analytically overwhelming, so the collected data were aggregated and simplified in several ways. The 20 social relationships were categorized into three groups for these analyses: self-provisioning, cooperative-provisioning, and social-provisioning (Fig. 3). Food could flow into households through any of the provisioning processes—own, cooperative, or social—but when food was redistributed to other households, it became—from the perspective of the receiving household—social-provisioning.
To further simplify and visualize flows of wild foods among households for some analyses, households were categorized into high-, medium-, and low-harvest terciles, and individual households’ harvests were aggregated to each tercile. In the aggregated data set, nodes represent groups of households, while the edges between nodes represent the aggregate sharing and cooperative wild flows moving between groups of households. For Wainwright and Kaktovik, whaling crews were categorized as a fourth type of node group (labeled “Crews”). The total harvests from successful crews (in pounds) across all social relationships were aggregated and then represented as flowing outward from a crew node to the three household-harvest terciles. Social and cooperative flows between harvest terciles (as well the crew nodes in Wainwright and Kaktovik) are presented as weighted, directed network diagrams, where edges reflect proportions of total flows in each community. Within-group flows—those that stay within a harvest tercile—were calculated as a proportion of total flows.
Representing equality
To visually represent and mathematically characterize the equalities of each community’s wild food system, we relied on Gini coefficients calculated from Lorenz curves. A Gini coefficient of zero represents perfect equality, where the resource of interest (income, wild food) is distributed equally among all households. A Gini coefficient of 1 represents perfect inequality, where the resource of interest rests entirely with one household. After calculating estimates of each household’s wild food inflows from each of the three types of provisioning relations, we drew Lorenz curves and calculated Gini coefficients using the “ineq” package in R (version 0.2-13) (Zeileis and Kleiber 2015, R Core Team 2021). We used these metrics to compare the cumulative distributions of households’ food inflows from each of the three categories of social relations redistribution (self-provisioning, cooperative-provisioning, social-provisioning). Note that equality at a community scale—objectively determined from quantitative observations or estimates of a variable—is not the same as equity.
Estimating influential flow factors
Based on northern ethnography and the work of Wolfe et al. (1987), we expected that wild food would flow from households with higher harvests and incomes to households with lower harvests and incomes, and from households with lower wild food needs to households with higher wild food needs. In addition, higher flows were expected between households with strong kin relationships than between households with extended or no kin relationships. To explore these ideas, we relied on a social relations regression model (Kenny and La Voie 1984) robust to zero inflation (Hoff et al. 2013, Hoff 2018). The social relations regression model regresses a dyadic outcome variable (directed, weighted flows of food between the nodes in the dyad) on a set of attributes of the source and the sink nodes of each dyad, as well as attributes shared by both nodes in each dyad.
In the regression models, the response variable was total social flows or total cooperative flows between pairs of households. A separate model was constructed for each community and each flow type (social or cooperative), for a total of six models. Predictor variables included node attributes total household harvest (in thousands of pounds), total household income (in tens of thousands of dollars), and a dichotomous “elder/single parent” household variable indicative of need. The “in-need” variable was set to 1 if a household consisted of a single parent (male or female) with children, or of elders living alone, elders raising children (one or two elders), or elders (one or two) with one adult and children (a variation on single parent), and zero otherwise (44 households across three communities had a value of 1). Elders were defined as those 60 years or older. Two dyadic variables were included to describe kin relationships between households. The “close kin” variable was set to 1 if a parent–child, sibling, and/or grandparent–grandchild relationship existed between a household pair, while the “extended kin” variable was set to 1 if a nepotic (aunt/uncle–niece/nephew), cousin, or in-law relationship existed. When both variables were zero, no kin relationship existed.
The ame function in the R package “amen” (version 1.3) (Hoff et al. 2017, R Core Team 2021) explicitly models dyadic relationships between row/source-specific and column/sink-specific node-level covariates and edge-level response variables (e.g., social relations regression models, as well as network models with higher order dependencies such as clustering and transitivity). The function uses a multivariate Gibbs sampling algorithm—a type of Markov chain Monte-Carlo sampler—to estimate model parameters. Details of the sampling algorithm and its implementation in ame can be found in Hoff (2015, 2018). Each community network was specified as a separate model, with dyadic and node-level predictors described in the preceding paragraphs. In total, 10,000 samples were drawn from the target (Tobit) distribution with a burn-in period of 500 samples. Default starting values were used, and model convergence was achieved. Posterior mean parameter estimates located in the tails of the distribution (i.e., less than 0.025 or greater than 0.975) were considered non-significant.
Qualitative data analysis
To explore the effect of sharing on well-being and, indirectly, on communities’ own perceptions of community equity, we relied on qualitative data from two survey questions posed only in Wainwright and Kaktovik, which differentiated between the effects of sharing at household and community levels. The questions were “How does sharing contribute to the well-being of your household?”, and “How does sharing contribute to the well-being of your community?” Qualitative data from open-ended responses were coded inductively to identify themes related to cultural narratives about sharing and cooperation and well-being in the context of subsistence (Charmaz 2006). A content analysis was used to summarize the frequency of themes mentioned by respondents. Some respondents provided multiple answers, so frequencies outnumbered respondents in some cases. For the household question, there were 152 responses in Wainwright and 60 responses in Kaktovik. For the community question, there were 142 responses in Wainwright and 65 responses in Kaktovik.
RESULTS
In the quantitative analyses section, we explore the community-level effects of cooperative-provisioning and social-provisioning on the equality of households’ access to wild food. Then we explore the magnitude of wild food flows among household harvest terciles in all three communities, and from whaling crews to terciles in Wainwright and Kaktovik. Finally, we model wild food flows on household- and dyad-level attributes contributing to equality and equity. In the qualitative analyses section, we summarize households’ responses about the perceived effects of sharing on household- and community-level well-being.
Quantitative analyses
Lorenz curves showed that self-provisioning was the least equally distributed type of inflow (curve farthest from the equality diagonal), meaning that many households provided very little wild food for themselves, while a few households provided a great deal (Fig. 4). For self-provisioning, the median household inflow as a percentage of total household inflows was very low—Wainwright: 1.8%; Kaktovik: 4.5%; Venetie: 11.5%. Cooperative-provisioning was slightly more equally distributed than self-provisioning, while social-provisioning was the most equally distributed (closest to the equality diagonal).
Gini coefficients in Fig. 4 were derived from the Lorenz curves and provide a unitary measure of the levels of equality for each of the three types of household inflows, as well as for all household inflows combined. The relatively high inequality of self-provisioning is reflected in Gini coefficients approaching 1.00, ranging from 0.79 to 0.84. Cooperative-provisioning inflows pushed wild food access toward equality among households, as reflected in reductions in the Gini coefficients over self-provisioning alone of 0.13, 0.09, and 0.05 for Wainwright, Kaktovik, and Venetie, respectively. The effects of social-provisioning inflows on equality were even stronger, as reflected in reductions over self-provisioning alone of 0.47, 0.28, and 0.18 in Wainwright, Kaktovik, and Venetie, respectively. The cumulative effect of social relationships reduced the Gini coefficient (i.e., increased equality) in all communities. The effect was strongest in Wainwright, where the Gini coefficient decreased from 0.79 for self-provisioning alone to 0.49 for all inflows (a 0.30 decrease), but was also evident in Kaktovik (0.19 decrease) and Venetie (0.17 decrease).
Visualizing wild food flows
By aggregating households and crews shown in Fig. 2 into harvest terciles and whaling crews, Fig. 5 provides a succinct summary of wild food flows in the three study communities. Cooperative and social flows among households in the same group are termed “within-group” flows, and are represented by looping edges.
The flows in Fig. 5 include three bowhead whales landed by Wainwright crews, accounting for 120,465 lb (54,642 kg), and three whales taken by Kaktovik crews, accounting for 84,616 lb (38,381 kg) across social- and cooperative-provisioning categories. Venetie does not hunt whales because of its inland location. Fig. 5 also includes the flows of other kinds of wild foods: 283,617 lb (128,647 kg) in Wainwright, 138,999 lb (63,049 kg) in Kaktovik, and 92,034 lb (41,746 kg) in Venetie. We discuss social-provisioning first, then cooperative-provisioning.
Social-provisioning
For whales, some social relationships are based on ideals of equal distribution across members of a community. This is expressed in community feasts (Captain’s feast and Nalukataq) and additionally household shares in Wainwright. In both communities, members of a household receive whaling feast shares if they choose, with a household of one receiving one share, and a household of 10 receiving 10 shares, etc. On average, Wainwright community members received 56.0 lb (25.4 kg) and Kaktovik received 48.3 lb (21.9 kg) per capita from both feasts. In contrast, sharing and the distribution of helper shares are culturally defined social flows that occur based on household-level relationships with others and specific contributions that households make to the whaling efforts of crews.
The effect of this mix of equality-based shares and household-specific roles and relationships on flows is apparent in Fig. 5. In Wainwright, 61,075 lb (27,703 kg) of social flows from crews were distributed across high-, medium-, and low-harvest groups (41%, 27%, and 22%, respectively). In Kaktovik, 7019 lb (3184 kg) of social flows from crews were distributed (60%, 34%, and 6%, respectively).
Social flows in Wainwright were much higher than social-provisioning in Kaktovik. There is an important caveat here, which emphasizes the flexibility of whaling efforts and social relationships overall. This was a result of a different pattern of distribution for the 2009 fall whale taken by one Wainwright crew. Rather than social-provisioning of whale occurring through feasts and some household-to-household ties—as occurred for all bowhead whales taken in Kaktovik—Wainwright’s fall whale was distributed as household shares immediately after landing. Since household shares are defined as social-provisioning, the proportion of whale flowing through social-provisioning relationships was higher in Wainwright than in prior years.
For species other than whales, social flows occurred between all harvest groups, and patterns differed across communities. In Wainwright, most social flows went to low-harvest households (45%) compared to 22% and 33% to high- and medium-harvest households. In Kaktovik and Venetie, most social flow went to high-harvest households (54% and 51%, respectively). Medium-harvest households received 39% and 30%, respectively, and low-harvest households received 8% and 19%, respectively. Households in all terciles were givers and receivers of social flows, but higher harvests correlated positively with inflow in Kaktovik and Venetie.
Within-group social-provisioning flows (social-provisioning of any resource among households in the same group) tracked groups’ harvest levels. Within-group social flows were highest for high-harvest groups in all communities, comprising 8% of all social-provisioning flows in Wainwright, 12% in Kaktovik, and 9% in Venetie. Within-group social flows were lowest for low-harvest groups, comprising 4.0% in Wainwright, 0.3% in Kaktovik, 0.6% in Venetie.
Cooperative-provisioning
For whales, cooperative flow patterns were determined by the shares allotted to umialgit and crew households. Households with captains and crew members fell almost entirely within high-harvest and medium-harvest groups (Fig. 5). In Wainwright, by far the largest portion of 64,302 lb (29,167 kg) of cooperative flows from whaling crews went to the high-harvest group (79%); much smaller portions went to the medium-harvest (18%) and low-harvest (3%) groups. A stronger pattern was evident for 65,321 lb (29,629 kg) of cooperative whale flows in Kaktovik, where the high-harvest group received 84%, the medium-harvest group 15%, and the low-harvest group 1%.
For species other than whales, cooperative-provisioning was also concentrated in high-harvest and medium-harvest groups. Cooperative-provisioning between high-harvest and medium-harvest households was particularly evident in Venetie, where medium-harvest households were a significant source of flows to the high-harvest group (Fig. 5). In all communities, low-harvest households had hunting relationships with high-harvest households, but the results emphasize productive hunting relationships between high-harvest households.
Compared with within-group social flows, within-group cooperative flows for high-harvest households were large. As a proportion of total community inflows, within-group cooperative flows totaled 28% in Wainwright, 41% in Kaktovik, and 21% in Venetie, again illustrating that highly productive households tended to hunt together. But medium- and low-harvest households also cooperated with other households in their harvest groups.
Modeling flow predictors
Across the three communities, social-provisioning accounted for 31% of all household inflows, while cooperative-provisioning (households working with members of other households and with crews) accounted for 43% of all household inflows. In this section, an additive multiplicative effects network regression model (Hoff 2018) is used to explore potentially influential predictors of wild food flows in social- and cooperative-provisioning. Unlike the previous section, which explored flows from crews and between aggregated groups of households, this section explores flows between household pairs (dyads) (see Methods and Fig. 2). We discuss social-provisioning first, and then cooperative-provisioning. Regression results, including posterior mean estimates and standard deviations, are shown in Fig. 6.
Modeling social-provisioning
The average social-provisioning inflow per household was 927 lb (420 kg) (25% of total inflow) in Wainwright, 1051 lb (477 kg) (22%) in Kaktovik, and 432 lb (196 kg) (37%) in Venetie. The difference between Wainwright and Kaktovik, on the one hand, and Venetie on the other, was a function of much larger cooperative-provisioning associated with the whaling complex. Close kin relationships and source household harvests were influential in all three communities. The significance and strength of other social-provisioning predictors varied by community.
The existence of a close kin relationship (parent–child, sibling, and grandparent–grandchild) between a source and sink household was the most influential predictor of social-provisioning flows across all three communities. A close kin relationship between a pair of households was associated with an average increase in social-provisioning flows of 89 lb (40 kg) in Wainwright, 322 lb (146 kg) in Kaktovik, and 175 lb (79 kg) in Venetie.
The magnitude of a household’s wild food harvest was the other influential, cross-community predictor of social-provisioning flows. As would be expected, households with higher harvests had higher social outflows. For each 1000-lb (454-kg) increase in the total harvest of a source household, social outflows would be expected to increase on average by 24 lb (11 kg) in Wainwright, 29 lb (13 kg) in Kaktovik, and 25 lb (11 kg) in Venetie. In Wainwright, household harvests were influential in another way: a 1000-lb (454-kg) increase in the total harvest of a sink household was associated with a 13-lb (6-kg) decrease in social inflows to that household. In Kaktovik and Venetie, sink household harvests were not significant influences on sink household inflows.
In Wainwright, elder or single-parent households were expected to give less than average to other households, a decrease of 89 lb (40 kg) in social outflows. In Venetie, elder and single-parent households were expected to receive more than average from other households, an increase of 37 lb (17 kg) in social inflows.
In summary, there was evidence of (1) a strong effect of close kinship on flows of shared wild food, (2) ability-based flows of food through social relationships (i.e., the more you have, the more you give) in Wainwright and Venetie, (3) need-based sharing related to harvest (i.e., the less you harvest, the more you receive) in Wainwright, and (4) need-based sharing related to single parent/elder status in Venetie.
Modeling cooperative-provisioning
The average cooperative-provisioning inflow was 1326 lb (604 kg) per household (44% of total inflow) in Wainwright,1681 lb (762 kg) (48%) in Kaktovik, and 291 lb (132 kg) (25%) in Venetie. As with social-provisioning, the higher flow magnitudes in the two Iñupiaq communities reflected the whaling complex. Because of the cooperative nature of the whaling complex, an additional household attribute—the number of captains or crew members in the household—was added to the cooperative model. The general pattern of influence for cooperative flows resembled the pattern for social-provisioning—source household harvests were again influential—but there were differences.
Compared to unrelated or extended kin households, cooperative flows (pounds) were on average 214 lb (97 kg) and 115 lb (52 kg) higher between closely related households in Kaktovik and Venetie, respectively. Close kinship was not a significant predictor of cooperative flows in Wainwright.
Households’ wild food harvests were a highly significant predictor of cooperative flows but were only modestly influential. For each 1000-lb (454-kg) increase in total harvest by the source household, the average amount of cooperative inflow would be expected to increase by 13 lb (6 kg) in Wainwright, 12 lb (5 kg) in Kaktovik, and 13 lb (6 kg) in Venetie. For a 1000-lb (454-kg) increase in total harvest of the sink household, the average amount of cooperative inflow would be expected to increase by 23 lb (10 kg), 19 lb (9 kg), and 15 lb (7 kg) in Wainwright, Kaktovik, and Venetie, respectively.
Compared with other household types, sink households headed by an elder/single parent received on average 91 and 93 fewer pounds of cooperative inflow in Wainwright and Kaktovik, respectively. This result suggests that elder and single parent households may be less likely to engage in cooperative hunting activities due to age and time constraints. Elder/single parent household type was not a significant predictor of cooperative flows in Venetie.
The number of whaling captains and/or crew members in a household was not a significant predictor of cooperative flows for source or sink households in Wainwright or Kaktovik. Finally, an increase of US$10,000 in total income of the source household in Wainwright and Venetie would be expected to increase cooperative inflows by 4 lb (2 kg) and 9 lb (4 kg), respectively. In Venetie, the same increase in income of the sink household would be expected to increase cooperative inflows by 5 lb (2 kg).
Qualitative analyses
In response to open-ended questions, respondents expanded on contributions of sharing to the well-being of communities and households (Fig. 7).
Four themes were expressed at household and community levels in both communities. By far, the most frequent theme for household-level effects of sharing on well-being was that sharing “feels good”. The reasons underlying these positive feelings were diverse. One elder described it this way: “I don’t know, I just…some say it’s respect and some say it just makes you feel good because you are helping others.” This idea also strongly resonated at the community level, signaling that sharing translates into broader feelings of good will.
Two other common themes—sharing provides food that improves health, and sharing maintains tradition—expressed that sharing improved well-being at both household and community levels. Community-level effects on “health” (respondents mentioned nutritional, physical, and psychological benefits) were more frequently mentioned in both communities, and Kaktovik respondents emphasized the role of sharing in maintaining tradition at the community rather than household level.
A fourth theme in common emphasized community cohesion. Some respondents described sharing as contributing to household well-being by strengthening community cohesion. Less surprisingly, sharing was more frequently described as a community-level mechanism that increased social cohesion, and therefore well-being. Responses to this question also highlighted a link between social cohesion and cultural integrity, as expressed by a woman in Wainwright: “sharing makes us stronger because it says who we are as Iñupiaq.”
There were differences in perceptions of sharing effects by community and level. Two infrequent themes expressed negative effects. A few respondents in both communities stated that sharing had no effect on their well-being because it did not occur frequently or at high enough magnitudes. A few Wainwright respondents similarly expressed the view that “no sharing” at the household level occurs.
Almost 16% of Wainwright responses highlighted the reciprocal nature of sharing as benefiting household well-being because “you give and later you receive.” Approximately 3% of Wainwright households described similar positive effects at the community level.
Three themes specific to Kaktovik emerged. At the household level, 8% of Kaktovik respondents said that sharing gives them a sense of pride. The reciprocal of this sentiment was expressed at the community level by 5% of Kaktovik respondents as they “gain respect in the community through sharing” with others. Finally, 11% of Kaktovik respondents described sharing as contributing to a complete community. “Sharing defines us as a household and a member of the community,” one Kaktovik respondent said. “It is very spiritual. It contributes both to economic aspects of the community, but also the spiritual.”
DISCUSSION
We integrated quantitative and qualitative observations from three Arctic communities to explore social resilience through the lens of cooperative production and distribution processes for wild foods at household and community levels. Our analyses are unusual in the Arctic subsistence literature in that we relied on households’ own reports of the amounts of wild food flowing through households’ social networks. We explored two propositions. First, sharing and cooperation are distributional processes that increase the equality of access to wild foods at the community level. Second, sharing and cooperation embody cultural mechanisms that express trust and build social cohesion.
The links between sharing and cooperative institutions, on the one hand, and social resilience in northern communities, on the other, hinge on the premise that equality, equity, and social cohesion are critical components of social resilience. A recent National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine (2021:8) report emphasized this connection: “Social capital and connectedness are among the most important factors in determining the resilience of a community”. Similarly, Michi Saagiig Nishnaabeg scholar Leanne Betasamosake Simpson (2017:77) invoked the resilience of her ancestors, who:
…didn’t accumulate capital, they accumulated networks of meaningful, deep, fluid, intimate collective and individual relationships of trust. In times of hardship, we did not rely to any great degree on accumulated capital or individualism but on the strength of our relationships with others.”
Simpson (2017) highlights both the material role of social networks in resilience, where resources physically move between people to potentially increase equality and equity, and the cultural role of social relationships, where the lived integration of human–animal relationships and processes of cooperation and sharing translate into greater shared capacity to respond to change. “Without a doubt,” Mishler and Frank (2019:239) wrote, “sharing and togetherness are what have made the Gwich’in so resilient over the centuries.” We described the roles of sharing and cooperation in social resilience in both contexts: material and cultural.
Quantitative analyses identified the material flows of wild foods among households through culturally resonant social relations, whih made it possible to assess the equality of households’ food access at the community scale. We illustrated quantitative patterns of social and cooperative flows between harvest groups, paying particular attention to the unique role of whaling crews in providing food and distributing it widely. We explored mechanisms of ability- and need-based household-to-household sharing that gave rise to increased equality of wild food distribution in the community. Qualitative analyses (Fig. 7) highlighted cultural and emotional effects of social ties on household and community well-being, which we applied as a meaningful proxy for social resilience (Healey Akearok et al. 2019). Our results emphasized that sharing and cooperation are social mechanisms that both increase equality and generate positive feelings and community cohesion in communities.
One important caveat in framing our results is that the climatic, social, and economic conditions that contributed to harvests and social ties for 2009–2010 describe only 1 year in a long continuum of social and subsistence time. These complexities are important to consider as communities in the Arctic and elsewhere adapt to an Arctic that is moving “faster now” than in the past (Krupnik and Jolly 2002).
Lorenz curves (Fig. 4) showed that wild foods flowing through social relations increased the equality of access at the community level in all three communities. Successive shifts toward equality in the social- and cooperative-provisioning curves reflect the cumulative positive effect of social relationships on wild food access. Gini coefficients (Fig. 4) quantified the magnitudes of these changes, showing substantial and positive shifts toward equality at the community level. Social relations did not achieve parity, nor does history suggest that they ever have (Burch 1980, Buela 2020). However, household-level sharing did result in greater equality at the community level, a point often assumed but rarely quantified. In all communities, social-provisioning resulted in the largest shift toward equality of wild food access, followed by cooperative-provisioning. The sizes of effects differed among communities. Total inflows were distributed most equally in Wainwright, followed by Venetie and Kaktovik (bottom row, Fig. 4).
High-harvest households were important mechanisms for wild food redistribution based on aggregated flows among three household-harvest groups and from whaling crews (Fig. 5). In the two Iñupiaq whaling communities, flows from whaling crews exemplified cultural premises of distribution throughout communities. High-harvest households typically included one or more crew members, and thus became sources to redistribute parts of crew and/or captain’s shares. High-harvest households (the top one-third) harvested most of the wild food in every community and (notably) engaged in cooperative harvests with other high-harvest households. They were less likely to engage in cooperative harvests with medium- and low-harvest households, but high-harvest households did contribute a large portion of the social flows to medium- and low-harvest households. Beyond simple distribution, the cultural patterns that provision and sustain crews and bring together hundreds of people onshore to bless, divide, cook, and share a landed bowhead whale also enact processes of social cohesion, well-being, and goodwill that characterize social resilience. One young whaling captain from Wainwright characterized the connections this way:
“My house and my cellar are open…ask me, tell me [you’ve taken some], just so I know it’s not there anymore, and I know I need to get more. I expect just a smile on their face. It meant life and death in the day. Now that’s not true, but it’s still really important…A community wouldn’t be a community without sharing. We wouldn’t be Wainwright.”
High-harvest households, then, were the foundational providers in the study communities through whaling, non-whaling cooperative hunting, and self-provisioning, production that is later shared through social-provisioning. Qualitative responses emphasize these feel-good, pride, and cultural strength effects of sharing at both household and community scales. Wolfe (1987) and Wolfe et al. (2009) showed that household harvests tended to be highly skewed toward a small number of “super households.” Using the corpus of comprehensive subsistence household survey data for Alaska at the time, Wolfe et al. (2009) modeled influential factors for household harvest levels and made the reasonable assumption that foods tended to flow out of high-harvest households and into low- and middle-harvest households. But in both studies (Wolfe 1987, Wolfe et al. 2009), the sources or destinations of wild foods given and received, and the quantities involved were unknown. In our study, the sources, destinations, and flow amounts were known. In the next section, we summarize our models of factors that influenced food flows among households rather than harvests by individual households as in Wolfe (1987).
Modeling food flows
Network regression provided quantitative evidence that supported existing narratives of wild food distribution between households on the basis of kinship, ability, need, and inclusivity (Fig. 7). We discuss these key factors and attributes that signal both distributional equity and equality—institutional patterns that are important for social resilience.
Kinship was a significant predictor of social-provisioning between households in all communities. Households shared more wild food with close kin than with extended family or non-kin. Knowing relatives and family lineages—iḷisimałiq iḷagiił̣iġmik—along with maintaining relationships with and caring for close family, are important values in Iñupiaq (Topkok 2015) and Dene cultures (Alaska Native Knowledge Network 2006). Previous studies highlighted the importance of kinship in the distribution of wild food for Canadian communities (e.g., Dombrowski et al. 2013a, b, Harder and Wenzel 2012, Natcher 2015) and the Nganasan and Dolgan of northern Siberia (Ziker 2007). Whether close or extended kin, our findings were in strong agreement with the narratives of sharing with relatives.
Total household harvest was a significant predictor of social outflows (“the more you have, the more you give”). Household harvest was also a significant predictor of cooperative flows, as high-harvest households tended to harvest wild food with each other. Sharing based on ability is part of being a good provider, which is a strong value in Iñupiaq (Bodenhorn 2000, Topkok and Green 2014, Hervé 2015), Dene cultures (Barnaby et al. 1977), and other northern Indigenous communities (Hovelsrud-Broda 1999, Usher et al. 2003, Natcher et al. 2021). High-harvest households in our study exemplified this pattern. Distribution patterns supported the idea that “the more you give, the more you receive,” which also indicated that generalized reciprocity (Sahlins 1972)—with humans and other non-human relations—is an important dimension of harvesting and sharing wild food. In Wainwright and Venetie, households with higher incomes tended to have slightly higher cooperative outflows, possibly a result of their ability to provision fuel or food for harvesting activities.
The model suggested that other households’ needs for wild foods was less influential than kinship and harvest factors but was still statistically significant. In Wainwright, households with lower total harvests and higher incomes received more in social inflows, while in Venetie and Kaktovik, elder/single parent households received more in social inflows. Households with these characteristics are often cited as those with whom wild food is shared first and frequently (Ziker 2002). Evidence of sharing to reduce inequities in access to traditional foods for elders and single mothers exists for Iñuit, as well as for Dene communities across the North (e.g., Kishigami 2000, Gombay 2010, McMillan and Parlee 2013). In Wainwright, higher household income predicted slightly higher social inflows. High-income household heads may have jobs that conflict with hunting or gathering (Collings et al. 1998), and high-income households may be able to purchase fuel or food for other households’ harvesting activities and receive wild food shares in return (Wenzel et al. 2000, Ready and Power 2018).
The strength and significance of elder/single parent status and income as predictors of receiving wild food varied, which may reflect variation in individual households or the changing nature of “need” itself. For example, some elders and single parents remain active providers and are not “in need” despite their elder/single parent status. Additionally, what “in need” means may be changing in modern mixed economies where income can mitigate against lack of wild food (Ready 2016, Willson 2016). However, we did find evidence that wild foods flowed to those households that were not able to provision enough wild food, whatever the reason.
An unexpected finding—given whaling’s substantial contributions to equality and equity in the two Iñupiaq communities—was that household members’ participation in whaling crews (as captains or crew members) was not a significant predictor of households’ wild food flows, either as sources or as sinks. For that, the whaling complex itself may be partly responsible. In the two whaling communities, every household receives whale shares irrespective of economic or social factors, which contributes to inclusivity and social connectedness. In essence it says, “It doesn’t matter who you are or who you know, the whale harvest benefits everyone.” Social connectedness is enhanced by feasts like Nalukataq and captain’s feasts, which both honor those who provide food for the community and enact the social ideal of provisioning others (MacLean, unpublished manuscript). While Venetie did not participate in whaling, studies in other Dene communities have reported similarly positive outcomes from cooperative caribou and moose hunting; e.g., social connectedness, public appreciation for hunters, and increased access to wild food (McMillan and Parlee 2013).
Social resilience and longitudinal heterogeneity
By definition, social resilience is a longitudinal process characterized by diversity and disruption. Over time, groups of individuals, households, and communities encounter and grapple with diverse challenges, and outcomes vary. In periods of uncertainty and transition, cooperative institutions of wild food production and distribution act as cultural and emotional “muscle” memories. They support and guide people as they cohere around a set of problems.
A recent high-profile Arctic example is the watershed 2018 ruling at the International Whaling Commission in support of a “limited automatic renewal” for the Indigenous hunting quota for bowhead whale, which removed the need for the Alaska Eskimo Whaling Commission to defend their hunting quota every 4 years (Koenig 2018, Scruggs 2018). The success reflected the strength of collaborative social relationships built over 40 years between whaling captains and their crews, the Alaska Eskimo Whaling Commission, leadership and scientific expertise from the North Slope Borough, and US representatives from the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, a co-management alliance that was grounded at every step in foundational Iñupiat values of sharing, cooperation, spirituality, and hard work (Topkok 2015, US Department of Commerce 2018).
On a smaller scale, communities with high levels of social capital across a range of contexts have been shown to experience less severe negative impacts from exposures. In an urban context, Klinenberg (1999) documented fewer deaths from record-setting heat in Chicago in neighborhoods with stronger social capital, and Lins et al. (2017) found higher corporate returns for socially responsible companies in the wake of the 2008 financial crisis. Further, MacLean et al. (2014:149) observed that “in times of change, [community-based social] networks provide essential support, operationalize community capacity, identify opportunities, and provide a focus for renewed optimism and hope”. Our results and those of previous studies suggest that people who repeatedly exercise relationships of cooperation, distribution, and sharing can address challenges more effectively and equitably than those who lack such a tradition of cooperation.
These results align with Aldrich and Meyer’s (2015) assertion that social infrastructure, not just physical infrastructure, is critical for community resilience. Berkes’ (2007) work in the Canadian Arctic identified a mechanism for this effect, wherein sharing networks contribute to community adaptive capacity in the face of shocks because they are storehouses for knowledge and experience of past generations’ responses to uncertainty. Day-to-day actions in social networks express these values under typical circumstances but can be leveraged into broader responses under more severe or novel conditions.
As households in northern communities engage with cash-generating opportunities, one would expect equality and equity outcomes across communities to change as well. Erickson (2021) points out that while communities across the North may be dealing with similar climatic or economic challenges, household diversity within and between communities is significant, so experiences and outcomes of change will be similarly diverse. Household heterogeneity across a range of socioeconomic and subsistence attributes theoretically indicative of sensitivity and adaptive capacity is being increasingly emphasized by northern scholars (Natcher 2009, Haalboom and Natcher 2012, Ready and Power 2018, BurnSilver and Magdanz 2019, Scaggs et al. 2021). Counter to this recognition of growing diversity is a tendency to universally categorize all northern communities as “vulnerable”. This assumption is problematic because it homogenizes agency (Haalboom and Natcher 2012) and erases important inter- and intra-community differences—such as the ability to respond to challenges by drawing on unique social networks—that set the stage for social resilience over time. Our results suggest that the loss of high-harvest households in the community could threaten community social resilience. However, this ebb and flow of highly active individuals and crews in communities due to employment, age, and health is also foundational to mixed economies and subsistence livelihoods.
The previous point highlights that longitudinal analyses of community networks are needed to explore levels of dynamism inherent in social institutions given cumulative patterns of change (Baggio et al. 2016, Ready and Power 2018). The connections drawn between social relationships, equity, equality, and social resilience for Wainwright, Kaktovik, and Venetie are based on data that describe patterns for one 12-month period, which masks expected variability (and flexibility) in social ties year-to-year. Longitudinal network research of this kind is rare (Janssen et al. 2006, Matous and Todo 2015, Ready and Power 2018), but it has the potential to resonate strongly with local narratives of good and bad years, response, change, and adjustment.
CONCLUSION
The Arctic social-ecological-systems literature is replete with conceptual papers positing linkages between community resilience and the social capital embedded in networks of relationships. Our goal was more focused: using both quantitative and qualitative data, we sought to empirically explore the effects of sharing and cooperation in three Arctic communities on equality and equity of wild food access, and through the repeated expression of social institutions that support these flows, greater trust and social cohesion. Our results strongly support the connection between equality, social resilience, and social institutions expressed as wild food flows and ties that connect Iñupiaq and Gwich’in to each other, to the animals they depend on, and to the places where they live. The question of social resilience of Arctic communities is much larger than just these attributes, inclusive of governance outcomes; political and economic trends; social, health, and employment challenges; and climate change—all of which change over time. However, community networks of sharing, cooperation, and trust continue to emerge as foundational in the broader efforts to navigate change.
These social institutions have evolved for tens of thousands of years, shaped subtly and sometimes aggressively by climate, conflicts, values, development initiatives, and politics. Sharing and cooperation networks—and the mix of skewed, equal, and equitable access to wild food they embody—persist today, and will continue to shape and support adaptation and community resilience into the future.
RESPONSES TO THIS ARTICLE
Responses to this article are invited. If accepted for publication, your response will be hyperlinked to the article. To submit a response, follow this link. To read responses already accepted, follow this link.AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Shauna BurnSilver and James Magdanz collected and analyzed data and wrote the manuscript. Jesse Coleman analyzed data and wrote the manuscript.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We express our sincere thanks to community partners in Wainwright, Kaktovik and Venetie—the Traditional Councils and Sharing Project Steering Committee members—who guided the project, hosted research teams, reviewed findings, and collaborated on dissemination of results. To the local interviewers who partnered on collecting data, and individuals in all three communities who gave their time and knowledge during interviews, we say “thank you”. The authors have learned a great deal about bowhead whaling and the meaning of whaling within Arctic mixed economies from ongoing communication and iteration with Commissioners and Counsel from the Alaska Eskimo Whaling Commission. This study was undertaken in response to the Bureau of Ocean Energy Management (BOEM), formerly Mineral Management Services Statement of Work NSL-AK-05-04 and was supported as grant #OCS Study BOEM 2015-023 DOI.
DATA AVAILABILITY
Data/code are openly available in a public repository that issues data sets with DOIs.
The data/code that support the findings of this study are openly available in anonymized and aggregated form from the Arctic Data Center at Household sharing and cooperation network and kinship data: Kaktovik, Wainwright, and Venetie, Alaska; 2009-2010. Reference number urn:uuid:31b56982-a9b2-4350-956c-7cc6bb260e87. Ethical approval for this research study was granted by the University of Alaska Fairbanks Institutional Research Board, approval number 05-70.
LITERATURE CITED
Active, J. 1998. Why subsistence is a matter of cultural survival: a Yup’ik point of view. Cultural Survival Quarterly 22(3).
Adger, W. 2000. Social and ecological resilience: Are they related? Progress in Human Geography 24:347-364. https://doi.org/10.1191/030913200701540465
Adger, W. N., N. W. Arnell, and E. L. Tompkins. 2005. Successful adaptation to climate change across scales. Global Environmental Change 15(2):77-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2004.12.005
Alaska Department of Fish & Game. 2020. ADF&G, Division of Subsistence, Community Subsistence Information System. https://www.adfg.alaska.gov/sb/CSIS/index.cfm?ADFG=main.conversionFactorSelRes
Alaska Native Knowledge Network. 2006. Athabascan cultural values. http://ankn.uaf.edu/ancr/Values/athabascan.html
Aldrich, D. P., and M. A. Meyer. 2015. Social capital and community resilience. American Behavioral Scientist 59(2):254-269. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764214550299
Alexie, E. D. 2015. “Nakhwanh Gwich’in Khehłok Iidilii – we are our own people”: Teetł’it Gwich’in practices of indigeneity: connection to land, traditional self-governance, and elements of self-determination. Thesis. University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada. http://hdl.handle.net/1828/6486
Baggio, J. A., S. B. BurnSilver, A. Arenas, J. S. Magdanz, G. P. Kofinas, and M. De Domenico. 2016. Multiplex social ecological network analysis reveals how social changes affect community robustness more than resource depletion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113(48):13708-13713. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1604401113
Barnaby, G., G. Kurszewski, and G. Cheezie. 1977. The political system and the Dene. Pages 120-129 in M. Watkins, editor. Dene Nation: the colony within. University of Toronto Press, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. https://doi.org/10.3138/9781487574451-015
Berkes, F. 2007. Understanding uncertainty and reducing vulnerability: lessons from resilience thinking. Natural Hazards 41:283-295. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11069-006-9036-7
Berkes, F., and H. Ross. 2013. Community resilience: toward an integrated approach. Society & Natural Resources 26(1):5-20. https://doi.org/10.1080/08941920.2012.736605
Berman, M., G. Kofinas, and S. BurnSilver. 2017. Measuring community adaptive and transformative capacity in the Arctic context. Pages 59-75 in G. Fondahl and G. N. Wilson, editors. Northern sustainabilities: understanding and addressing change in the circumpolar world. Springer International Publishing, Cham, Switzerland. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-46150-2_6
Bernard, H. R. 2006. Research methods in anthropology. AltaMira Press, New York, USA.
Betasamosake Simpson, L. 2017. As we have always done: Indigenous freedom through radical resistance. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, Minnesota, USA.
Bodenhorn, B. 1990. “I’m not the great hunter, my wife is”: Iñupiat and anthropological models of gender. Études/Inuit/Studies 14(1):55-74.
Bodenhorn, B. 2000. “It’s good to know who your relatives are, but we were taught to share with everybody”: shares and sharing among Iñupiaq households. Senri Ethnological Studies 53:27-60.
Borgatti, S. P., A. Mehra, D. J. Brass, and G. Labianca. 2009. Network analysis in the social sciences. Science 323(5916):892-895. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1165821
Brown, K. 2014. Global environmental change I: a social turn for resilience? Progress in Human Geography 38(1):107-117. https://doi.org/10.1177/0309132513498837
Brown, K., and E. Westaway. 2011. Agency, capacity, and resilience to environmental change: lessons from human development, well-being, and disasters. Annual Review of Environment and Resources 36(1):321-342. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-environ-052610-092905
Burch, E. S. 1975. Eskimo kinsmen: changing family relationships in northwest Alaska. West Publishing Company, St. Paul, Minnesota, USA.
Buela, A. 2020. Leadership and inequality among the Iñuipat: a case of transegalitarian hunter-gatherers. In L. Moreau, editor. Social inequality before farming? Multidisciplinary approaches to the study of social organization in prehistoric and ethnographic hunter-gatherer-fisher societies. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
Burch, E. S. 1980. Traditional Eskimo societies in Northwest Alaska. Senri Ethnological Studies 4:253-304.
BurnSilver, S. B., R. B. Boone, G. P. Kofinas, and T. J. Brinkman. 2017. Modeling tradeoffs in a rural Alaska mixed economy: hunting, working, and sharing in the face of economic and ecological change. Pages 52-83 in M. Hegmon, editor. The give and take of sustainability: archaeological and anthropological perspectives on tradeoffs. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781139939720.004
BurnSilver, S., and J. Magdanz. 2019. Heterogeneity in mixed economies: implications for sensitivity and adaptive capacity. Hunter Gatherer Research 3(4):601-633. https://doi.org/10.3828/hgr.2017.31
BurnSilver, S., J. Magdanz, R. Stotts, M. Berman, and G. Kofinas. 2016. Are mixed economies persistent or transitional? Evidence using social networks from Arctic Alaska. American Anthropologist 118(1):121-129. https://doi.org/10.1111/aman.12447
Burwell, M. 2005. Hunger knows no law: seminal Native protest and the Barrow duck-in of 1961. International Congress of Arctic Social Scientists, Fairbanks, Alaska, USA.
Charmaz, K. 2006. Constructing grounded theory: a practical guide through qualitative research. Sage Publications Ltd, London, UK.
Collings, P., M. G. Marten, T. Pearce, and A. G. Young. 2016. Country food sharing networks, household structure, and implications for understanding food insecurity in Arctic Canada. Ecology of Food and Nutrition 55(1):30-49. https://doi.org/10.1080/03670244.2015.1072812
Collings, P., G. Wenzel, and R. G. Condon. 1998. Modern food sharing networks and community integration in the central Canadian Arctic. Arctic 51(4):301-314. https://doi.org/10.14430/arctic1073
Cuthill, M., H. Ross, K. Maclean, K. Owens, B. Witt, and C. King. 2008. Reporting social outcomes of development: an analysis of diverse approaches. International Journal of Interdisciplinary Social Sciences 3(6):145-157. https://doi.org/10.18848/1833-1882/CGP/v03i06/52646
Dacks, R., T. Ticktin, S. D. Jupiter, and A. M. Friedlander. 2020. Investigating the role of fish and fishing in sharing networks to build resilience in coral reef social-ecological systems. Coastal Management 48(3):165-187. https://doi.org/10.1080/08920753.2020.1747911
Damas, D. 1972. Central Eskimo systems of food sharing. Ethnology 11(3):220-240. https://doi.org/10.2307/3773217
Dombrowski, K., E. Channell, B. Khan, J. Moses, and E. Misshula. 2013a. Out on the land: income, subsistence activities, and food sharing networks in Nain, Labrador. Journal of Anthropology 2013:1-11. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/185048
Dombrowski, K., B. Khan, E. Channell, J. Moses, K. McLean, and E. Misshula. 2013b. Kinship, family, and exchange in a Labrador Inuit community. Arctic Anthropology 50(1):89-104. https://doi.org/10.3368/aa.50.1.89
Donkersloot, R., J. Coleman, C. Carothers, D. Ringer, and P. Cullenberg. 2020. Kin, community, and diverse rural economies: rethinking resource governance for Alaska rural fisheries. Marine Policy 117:103966.
Duhaime, G., E. Searles, P. J. Usher, H. Myers, and P. Fréchette. 2004. Social cohesion and living conditions in the Canadian Arctic: from theory to measurement. Social Indicators Research 66(3):295-318. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SOCI.0000003726.35478.fc
Erickson, K. R. S. 2020. A sharing culture. UAF Geophysical Institute TEK Talks. Fairbanks, Alaska, USA, November 10, 2020.
Erickson, K. R. S. 2021. Ingenious Indigenous technology and Inuit history. ARCUS Research Seminar, May 27, 2021. https://www.arcus.org/arctic-info/archive/32101
Gadamus, L., and J. Raymond-Yakoubian. 2015. Qualitative participatory mapping of seal and walrus harvest and habitat areas: documenting Indigenous knowledge, preserving local values, and discouraging map misuse. International Journal of Applied Geospatial Research 6(1):76-93. https://doi.org/10.4018/ijagr.2015010105
Gombay, N. 2010. Community, obligation, and food: lessons from the moral geography of Inuit. Geografiska Annaler: Series B, Human Geography 92(3):237-250. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0467.2010.00350.x
Haalboom, B., and D. C. Natcher. 2012. The power and peril of “vulnerability”: approaching community labels with caution in climate change research. Arctic 65(3):319-327. https://doi.org/10.14430/arctic4219
Harder, M. T., and G. W. Wenzel. 2012. Inuit subsistence, social economy and food security in Clyde River, Nunavut. Arctic 65(3):305-318. https://doi.org/10.14430/arctic4218
Healey Akearok, G., K. Cueva, J. P. A. Stoor, C. V. L. Larsen, E. Rink, N. Kanayurak, A. Emelyanova, and V. Y. Hiratsuka. 2019. Exploring the term “resilience” in Arctic health and well-being using a sharing circle as a community-centered approach: insights from a conference workshop. Social Sciences 8(2):45. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci8020045
Hervé, C. 2015. Le pouvoir vient d'ailleurs?: leadership et coopération chez les Inuits du Nunavik (Arctique québécois). Presses de l'Université Laval, Laval, Québec, Canada.
Hoff, P. D. 2015. Dyadic data analysis with amen. arXiv:1506.08237 [stat].
Hoff, P. D. 2018. Additive and multiplicative effects network models. arXiv:1807.08038 [stat]. https://doi.org/10.1214/19-STS757
Hoff, P., B. Fosdick, A. Volfovsky, and Y. He. 2017. Additive and multiplicative effects models for networks and relational data. R package version 1.4.4. https://cloud.r-project.org/package=amen
Hoff, P., B. Fosdick, A. Volfovsky, and K. Stovel. 2013. Likelihoods for fixed rank nomination networks. Network Science 1(3):253-277. https://doi.org/10.1017/nws.2013.17
Hovelsrud-Broda, G. 2000. “Sharing”, transfers, transactions and the concept of generalized reciprocity. Senri Ethnological Studies 53:193-214.
Hovelsrud-Broda, G. K. 1999. The integrative role of seals in an east Greenlandic hunting village. Arctic Anthropology 36(1/2):37-50.
Huntington, H. P. 1989. The Alaska Eskimo Whaling Commission: effective local management of a subsistence resource. University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.
Jacobson, S. A. 2012. Yup’ik Eskimo dictionary. Second edition. Alaska Native Language Center, Fairbanks, Alaska, USA.
Janssen, M. A., Ö. Bodin, J. M. Anderies, T. Elmqvist, H. Ernstson, R. R. McAllister, P. Olsson, and P. Ryan. 2006. Toward a network perspective of the study of resilience in social-ecological systems. Ecology and Society 11(1):15. http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol11/iss1/art15/
Johnson, J. S., E. D. Nobmann, E. Asay, and A. P. Lanier. 2009. Dietary intake of Alaska Native people in two regions and implications for health: the Alaska Native Dietary and Subsistence Food Assessment Project. International Journal of Circumpolar Health 68(2):109-122. https://doi.org/10.3402/ijch.v68i2.18320
Jolles, C. Z., and E. M. Oozeva. 2002. Faith, food, and family in a Yupik whaling community. University of Washington Press, Seattle, Washington, USA.
Kenny, D. A., and L. La Voie. 1984. The Social relations model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 18:141-182. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60144-6
Kishigami, N. 2000. Contemporary Inuit food sharing and hunter support program of Nunavik, Canada. Senri Ethnological Studies 53:171-192.
Kishigami, N. 2004. A new typology of food-sharing practices among hunter-gatherers, with a special focus on Inuit examples. Journal of Anthropological Research 60(3):341-358. https://doi.org/10.1086/jar.60.3.3630754
Kishigami, N. 2013. On sharing of bowhead whale meat and maktak in an Inupiat community of Barrow, Alaska, USA. Bulletin of the Hokkaido Museum of Northern Peoples 22:1-19.
Klinenberg, E. 1999. Denaturalizing disaster: a social autopsy of the 1995 Chicago heat wave. Theory and Society 28(2):239-295. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006995507723
Koenig, R. 2018. Reactions from Utqiaġvik on a whaling quota rule change: “We don’t have to beg anymore.” Alaska Public Media, Anchorage, Alaska, USA, December 6, 2018.
Kofinas, G., S. BurnSilver, J. Magdanz, R. Stotts, and M. Okada. 2016. Subsistence sharing networks and cooperation: Kaktovik, Wainwright, and Venetie, Alaska. US Bureau of Ocean Energy Management. https://arctichealth.org/media/pubs/303436/BOEM-2015-23.pdf
Koster, J. 2011. Interhousehold meat sharing among Mayangna and Miskito horticulturalists in Nicaragua. Human Nature 22(4):394-415. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-011-9126-4
Kral, M. J., L. Idlout, J. B. M. Ronald, J. Dyck, and L. J. Kirmayer. 2011. Unikkaartuit: meanings of well-being, unhappiness, health, and community change among Inuit in Nunavut, Canada. American Journal of Community Psychology 48(3-4):426-438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-011-9431-4
Krupnik, I., and D. Jolly, editors. 2002. The earth is faster now: Indigenous observations of Arctic environmental change. Arctic Research Consortium of the United States, Fairbanks, Alaska, USA.
Kruse, J., B. Poppel, L. Abryutina, G. Duhaime, S. Martin, M. Poppel, M. Kruse, E. Ward, P. Cochran, and V. Hanna. 2009. Survey of living conditions in the Arctic (SliCA). Pages 107-134 in V. Møller, D. Huschka, and A. C. Michalos, editors. Barometers of quality of life around the globe. Springer, Dordrecht, Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-8686-1_5
Langdon, S. J. 1991. The integration of cash and subsistence in southwest Alaska Yup’ik Eskimo communities. Pages 269-291 in T. Matsuyama and N. Peterson, editors. Cash, commoditization and changing foragers. Senri Ethnological Studies, Osaka, Japan.
Langdon, S., and R. Worl. 1981. Distribution and exchange of subsistence resources in Alaska. University of Alaska, Arctic Environmental Information and Data Center, Anchorage, Alaska, USA.
Leach, M., B. Reyers, X. Bai, E. S. Brondizio, C. Cook, S. Díaz, G. Espindola, M. Scobie, M. Stafford-Smith, and S. M. Subramanian. 2018. Equity and sustainability in the Anthropocene: a social-ecological systems perspective on their intertwined futures. Global Sustainability 1. https://doi.org/10.1017/sus.2018.12
Lin, N. 1999. Social networks and status attainment. Annual Review of Sociology 25(1):467-487. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.25.1.467
Lins, K. V., H. Servaes, and A. Tamayo. 2017. Social capital, trust, and firm performance: the value of corporate social responsibility during the financial crisis. Journal of Finance 72(4):1785-1824. https://doi.org/10.1111/jofi.12505
Maclean, K., M. Cuthill, and H. Ross. 2014. Six attributes of social resilience. Journal of Environmental Planning and Management 57(1):144-156. https://doi.org/10.1080/09640568.2013.763774
Magdanz, J. S., J. Greenberg, J. Little, and D. S. Koster. 2016. The persistence of subsistence: wild food harvests in rural Alaska, 1983-2013. Social Science Research Network. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2779464
Magdanz, J. S., C. J. Utermohle, and R. J. Wolfe. 2002. The production and distribution of wild food in Wales and Deering, Alaska. Alaska Department of Fish and Game, Division of Subsistence.
Matous, P., and Y. Todo. 2015. Exploring dynamic mechanisms of learning networks for resource conservation. Ecology and Society 20(2):36. https://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol20/iss2/art36/
McMillan, R., and B. Parlee. 2013. Dene hunting organization in Fort Good Hope, Northwest Territories: “Ways we help each other and share what we can.” Arctic 66(4):435-447. https://doi.org/10.14430/arctic4330
Mitchell, D. C. 2001. Take my land, take my life: the story of Congress’s historic settlement of Alaska Native Land Claims, 1960–1971. University of Alaska Press, Fairbanks, Alaska.
Mishler, C. and K. E. Frank. 2019. Dinjii Vadzaih Dhidlit: the man who became a caribou. International Polar Institute.
Natcher, D. C. 2009. Subsistence and the social economy of Canada’s Aboriginal North. Northern Review 30:83-98.
Natcher, D. C. 2015. Social capital and the vulnerability of Aboriginal food systems in Canada. Human Organization 74(3):230-242. https://doi.org/10.17730/0018-7259-74.3.230
Natcher, D. C., L. Felt, J. McDonald, and R. Ford. 2012. The social organization of wildfood production in Postville, Nunatsiavut. University of Manitoba Press, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada.
Natcher, D., S. Ingram, A.-M. Bogdan, and A. Rice. 2021. Conservation and Indigenous subsistence hunting in the Peace River region of Canada. Human Ecology 49(2):109-120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10745-020-00210-z
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2021. Enhancing community resilience through social capital and connectedness: stronger together! National Academies Press, Washington, D.C., USA.
Nolin, D. A. 2010. Food-sharing networks in Lamalera, Indonesia: reciprocity, kinship, and distance. Human Nature 21(3):243-268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-010-9091-3
Nolin, D. A. 2011. Kin preference and partner choice: patrilineal descent and biological kinship in Lamaleran cooperative relationships. Human Nature 22(1-2):156-176. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-011-9113-9
Nolin, D. A. 2012. Food-sharing networks in Lamalera, Indonesia: status, sharing, and signaling. Evolution and Human Behavior 33(4):334-345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.11.003
Okada, M. M. 2010. The comparison of qualitative and quantitative approaches for measuring traditional food sharing in communities on the North Slope of Alaska. Thesis. University of Alaska Fairbanks, Fairbanks, Alaska, USA.
Putnam, R. D. 2000. Bowling alone: the collapse and revival of American community. Simon and Schuster, New York, USA.
R Core Team. 2021. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/
Raymond-Yakoubian, J., and V. Angnaboogok. 2017. Cosmological changes: shifts in human-fish relationships in Alaska’s Bering Strait region. Pages 105-118 in T. Räsänen and T. Syrjämaa, editors. Shared lives of humans and animals: animal agency in the global north. Routledge, New York, USA. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315228761-11
Ready, E. 2016. Challenges in the assessment of Inuit food security. Arctic 69(3):266-280-266-280. https://doi.org/10.14430/arctic4579
Ready, E. 2018. Sharing-based social capital associated with harvest production and wealth in the Canadian Arctic. PLOS ONE 13(3):e0193759. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193759
Ready, E., and E. A. Power. 2018. Why wage earners hunt: food sharing, social structure, and influence in an Arctic mixed economy. Current Anthropology 59(1):74-97. https://doi.org/10.1086/696018
Reedy-Maschner, K. 2009. Entangled livelihoods: economic integration and diversity in the western Arctic. Alaska Journal of Anthropology 7(2):135-146.
Sahlins, M. 1972. Stone age economics. AldineTransaction, London, UK.
Scaggs, S. A., D. Gerkey, and K. R. McLaughlin. 2021. Linking subsistence harvest diversity and productivity to adaptive capacity in an Alaskan food sharing network. American Journal of Human Biology 2021:e23573. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23573
Scruggs, G. 2018. After decades of uncertainty, Native Alaskans celebrate whaling rights. Christian Science Monitor, Utqiaġvik, Alaska, December 5, 2018.
Seiler, W. A. 2012. Iñupiatun Eskimo dictionary. SIL International, Dallas, Texas, USA.
Smith, T. G., and H. Wright. 1989. Economic status and role of hunters in a modern Inuit village. Polar Record 25(153):93-98. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003224740001038X
State of Alaska. 2020. DCRA open data. Department of Commerce, Community, and Economic Development, Division of Community and Regional Affairs. https://dcra-cdo-dcced.opendata.arcgis.com/
Stephen R. Braund & Associates (SRB&A). 2018. Description of Alaskan Eskimo bowhead whale subsistence sharing practices: including an overview of bowhead whale harvesting and community-based need. Alaska Eskimo Whaling Commission, Anchorage, Alaska.
Thomas, D. S. G., and C. Twyman. 2005. Equity and justice in climate change adaptation amongst natural-resource-dependent societies. Global Environmental Change 15(2):115-124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2004.10.001
Thorton, T., and N. Mamontova. 2016. Hunter-gatherers and fishing rights in Alaska and Siberia: contemporary governmentality, subsistence, and sustainable enterprises. Pages 149-173 in V. Reyes-García and A. Phyälä, editors. Hunter-gatherers in a changing world. Springer International Publishing, Cham, Switzerland.
Topkok, C. S. A. 2015. Iñupiat Ilitqusiat: inner views of our Iñupiaq values. Thesis. University of Alaska Fairbanks, Fairbanks, Alaska, USA.
Topkok, C. S. A., and C. Green. 2014. Following the pathways of the ancestors: well-being through Iñupiaq dance. Pages 7-21 in F. Deer, T. Falkenberg, B. McMillan, and L. Sims, editors. Sustainable well-being: concepts, issues and educational practices. Education for Sustainable Well-Being Press, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada.
US Census Bureau. 2012. Alaska: 2010 population and housing unit count. US Department of Commerce: Economics and Statistics Administration.
US Department of Commerce, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration. 2018. Final environmental impact statement for issuing annual catch limits to the Alaska Eskimo Whaling Commission for a subsistence hunt on bowhead whales for the years 2019 and beyond. National Marine Fisheries Service, Alaska Region, Juneau, Alaska, USA.
Usher, P. J., G. Duhaime, and E. Searles. 2003. The household as an economic unit in Arctic Aboriginal communities, and its measurement by means of a comprehensive survey. Social Indicators Research 61:175-202. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021344707027
Webster, D., and W. Zibell. 1970. Iñupiat Eskimo dictionary. Alaska Rural School Project, University of Alaska, Fairbanks, Alaska, USA.
Wenzel, G. W. 2000. Sharing, money and modern Inuit subsistence: obligation and reciprocity at Clyde River, Nunavut. Senri Ethnological Studies 53:61-85.
Wenzel, G. W., G. Hovelsrud-Broda, and N. Kishigami. 2000. Introduction: social economy of modern hunter gatherers: traditional subsistence, new resources. Senri Ethnological Studies 53:1-6.
Willson, E. 2016. Exploring and evaluating personal, cultural and social food needs and the role of a community freezer among Inuit in Hopedale, Nunatsiavut. Thesis. Trent University, Peterborough, Ontario, Canada.
Wilson, N. J. 2014. The politics of adaptation: subsistence livelihoods and vulnerability to climate change in the Koyukon Athabascan village of Ruby, Alaska. Human Ecology 42(1):87-101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10745-013-9619-3
Wolfe, R. J. 1987. The “super-household” in Alaska Native subsistence economies. 14th annual meeting, Alaska Anthropological Association, Anchorage, Alaska, USA.
Wolfe, R. J., and R. J. Walker. 1987. Subsistence economies in Alaska: productivity, geography, and development impacts. Arctic Anthropology 24(2):56-81.
Wolfe, R. J., C. L. Scott, W. E. Simeone, C. J. Utermohle, and M. C. Pete. 2009. The “super-household” in Alaska Native subsistence economies. Final Report to the National Science Foundation ARC 352611.
Woolcock, M., and D. Narayan. 2000. Social capital: implications for development theory, research, and policy. World Bank Research Observer 15(2):225-249. https://doi.org/10.1093/wbro/15.2.225
Zeileis, A., and C. Kleiber. 2015. Measuring inequality, concentration, and poverty.
Ziker, J. P. 2002. Peoples of the tundra: northern Siberians in the post-Communist transition. Waveland Press, Long Grove, Illinois, USA.
Ziker, J. P. 2007. Subsistence and food sharing in northern Siberia: social and nutritional ecology of the Dolgan and the Nganasan. Ecology of Food and Nutrition 46(5-6):445-467. https://doi.org/10.1080/03670240701486743
Ziker, J. P. 2014. Sharing, subsistence, and social norms in northern Siberia. Pages 337-356 in J. Ensminger and J. Henrich, editors. Experimenting with social norms: fairness and punishment in cross-cultural perspective. Russell Sage Foundation, New York, USA.