Skip to content
Opens in a new window Opens an external site Opens an external site in a new window
Ecology & Society
  • Current Issue
  • About the Journal
    • Our Editors
    • Policies
    • Submissions
    • Contact
  • Open Access Policy
  • Submit an Article
  • Sign In
Icons/Search
Icons/Close
Icons/Search
Home > VOLUME 30 > ISSUE 4 > Article 3 Research

Relational values of nature—a global empirical study of environmental students in 37 countries

Kleespies, M. W., M. Hahn-Klimroth, and P. W. Dierkes. 2025. Relational values of nature—a global empirical study of environmental students in 37 countries. Ecology and Society 30(4):3. https://doi.org/10.5751/ES-16513-300403
Download PDF Download icon Download Citation Download icon Submit a Response Arrow-Forward icon
Share
  • Twitter logo
  • LinkedIn logo
  • Facebook logo
  • Email Icon
  • Link Icon
  • Matthias W. KleespiesORCIDcontact author, Matthias W. Kleespies
    Goehte University, Frankfurt, Germany
  • Max Hahn-KlimrothORCID, Max Hahn-Klimroth
    Goehte University, Frankfurt, Germany
  • Paul W. DierkesORCIDPaul W. Dierkes
    Goehte University, Frankfurt, Germany

The following is the established format for referencing this article:

Kleespies, M. W., M. Hahn-Klimroth, and P. W. Dierkes. 2025. Relational values of nature—a global empirical study of environmental students in 37 countries. Ecology and Society 30(4):3.

https://doi.org/10.5751/ES-16513-300403

  • Introduction
  • Methods
  • Results
  • Discussion
  • Implications
  • Limitations
  • Conclusion
  • Responses to this Article
  • Author Contributions
  • Acknowledgments
  • Use of Artificial Intelligence (AI) and AI-assisted Tools
  • Data Availability
  • Literature Cited
  • decision makers; environmental students; international survey; relational values; RVs; unsupervised learning; Ward’s clustering algorithm
    Relational values of nature—a global empirical study of environmental students in 37 countries
    Copyright © by the author(s). Published here under license by The Resilience Alliance. This article is under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. You may share and adapt the work provided the original author and source are credited, you indicate whether any changes were made, and you include a link to the license. ES-2025-16513.pdf
    Research

    ABSTRACT

    For decades, the question of why people want to protect nature was mostly answered from an instrumental or intrinsic perspective. However, in recent years, a new category of values has received attention: relational values (RVs). Relational values refer to meaningful relationships that people form with nature and with each other through nature, including ethical responsibilities, cultural significance, and identity. They go beyond the dichotomy of intrinsic and instrumental values by highlighting the role of care, stewardship, and responsibility in shaping human–nature relationships. Currently, there is a lack of quantitative empirical studies on the concept of RVs, especially in the international context. This study, therefore, surveyed 4571 environmental students in 37 countries to gain an overview of their RVs. A cluster analysis revealed that there are six overarching global evaluation patterns of RVs, so-called response types, that occur worldwide. The six response types show different characteristics and variations in the agreement of different elements of the RVs. These response types ranged from strong agreement with all relational value items (type 1), to selective endorsement (types 3–6), to broad rejection of RVs (type 2). The correlation of these response types with country-specific wealth indicators showed that the RVs are less pronounced in wealthy countries. This study is the first to carry out a large international comparison of RVs.

    INTRODUCTION

    At a time when environmental issues have already become major global problems, the question arises as to what influences people’s environmental behavior. The perception of the value of nature is seen as an important factor that can have a decisive influence on decision making (Pascual et al. 2023). In the past, a value dichotomy between the instrumental and intrinsic value of nature was often assumed (Tallis and Lubchenco 2014, Pascual et al. 2017). In the case of intrinsic value, nature and biodiversity are assigned a value simply because of their existence (Soulé 1985). This value is inalienable, i.e., it cannot be denied and is independent of any evaluation by humans. Nature should, therefore, be protected for its own sake (Soulé 1985, Sandler 2012). However, this view is often criticized: for example, protecting nature for its own sake is seen by some as outdated and impractical (Soulé 2014), and it is not clear whether this valuation of nature can really increase environmentally friendly behavior (Justus et al. 2009). Therefore, supporters of the instrumental view argue that the instrumental approach is easier for more people to understand than the intrinsic value of nature (Tallis and Lubchenco 2014). The instrumental perspective focuses on the benefits nature provides to humans rather than nature’s intrinsic worth (Bullock 2017). This perspective is closely linked to the ecosystem services from which humans benefit (Reyers et al. 2012). In the instrumental valuation of nature, a part of nature is replaceable if another or new part fulfils the same function (Sandler 2012). However, this view is also criticized by some as a sell-out of nature (McCauley 2006). It is also problematic that only parts of nature that have a benefit for humans are considered worthy of protection, which leaves out parts that have little or no obvious benefits (Tallis and Lubchenco 2014).

    Nowadays, value pluralism is increasingly discussed in environmental valuation as an approach to integrating diverse perspectives on nature’s worth. However, it remains an evolving and contested framework (Intergovernmental Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services (IPBES) 2018, Pascual et al. 2023). Value pluralism refers to the idea that multiple values coexist that cannot be reduced to a single overarching metric (Chang 2015). Although some authors see value pluralism as an important step for sustainability and as a way to integrate diverse perspectives on nature’s contributions to people, others criticize that privileged actors may have greater influence on which values are recognized (Pascual et al. 2017, Jacobs et al. 2020, 2023).

    Besides instrumental and intrinsic values of nature, another type of environmental values has also attracted increasing attention in recent years: the concept of relational values (RVs), in which human–nature relationships and nature-centered human relationships play an important role (Díaz et al. 2015). Relational values refer to meaningful relationships people form with nature and with each other through nature, encompassing ethical responsibilities, cultural significance, and identity. They extend beyond the conventional dichotomy of intrinsic and instrumental values by emphasizing the importance of relationships themselves (Chan et al. 2016, Pascual et al. 2017). These values are grounded in notions of stewardship, care, identity, and social cohesion, shaping how individuals and communities interact with and assign meaning to nature (Chan et al. 2018, Pratson et al. 2023). However, the term “RVs” is constantly evolving and may be used with slightly different nuances by authors (Pratson et al. 2023, Himes et al. 2024). Relational values provide an important lens for conservation by recognizing why people form attachments to places/nature and how these connections influence decision making (Himes and Muraca 2018, Pratson et al. 2023).

    The concept of RVs has been further refined in recent years and aligned with well-known environmental value concepts such as assigned, moral, and held values: RVs include assigned values (values of objects) and moral values (what is right). However, held values, which describe higher ideals or principles (e.g., fairness, generosity) and are more abstract, differ from RVs because RVs are always linked to concrete relationships involving nature (Chan et al. 2018). Relational values are increasingly recognized as crucial for sustainability, as they encompass and promote a sense of responsibility, care, and stewardship toward nature, supporting long-term environmental commitments (Martin et al. 2024). Relational values also play a significant role in shaping environmental behavior (van den Born et al. 2018, Topp et al. 2022, Uehara et al. 2022). In addition, they can serve as the main drivers and key motivators for participation in conservation activities (Knippenberg et al. 2018, Mattijssen et al. 2020). Even before the term was introduced, RVs or parts of them were identified in the environmental education sector, without naming them as such (Britto dos Santos and Gould 2018). However, the boundaries between RVs and instrumental and intrinsic values of nature are often blurred, as different types of values can be ascribed to or derived from the same part of nature (Himes et al. 2024, Riechers et al. 2025).

    Due to this great importance of RVs, research has been conducted in different regions of the world and in various scientific disciplines, often using different methods and objectives (Pratson et al. 2023). Although these studies provide valuable insights, there is a lack of broader, comparative approaches to identify universal and culturally specific aspects of RVs. Furthermore, the majority of existing quantitative RV assessments are conducted at a national or local scale, thereby creating a knowledge gap about the global perception of RVs. Further quantitative studies are, therefore, needed: for example, to identify elements within RVs that occur in different cultures (Schulz and Martin-Ortega 2018). A particularly interesting group in this context are students in the environmental and sustainability field.

    Universities play an important role when it comes to the development of sustainability. They educate future decision makers, leaders (Alshuwaikhat and Abubakar 2008, Bellou et al. 2017), professionals (Kioupi and Voulvoulis 2020), and intellectuals in the academic field (Lozano 2006). Although important positions in society can be achieved without university education, university education favors the achievement of such positions, as skills and knowledge are taught there (Vicente-Molina et al. 2013).

    How these future decision makers in the environmental sector evaluate RVs is largely unknown. At the moment, such studies only exist in a national context, if at all (Feucht et al. 2023). Therefore, especially for this group, it would be beneficial to find out more about their perception of RVs, as they are likely to make environmentally relevant decisions later in their career. Conducting surveys among students provides valuable insights into the perspectives of future environmental decision makers. However, the survey population is not without its limitations. Students may not fully represent the broader population due to the influence of their academic background or socioeconomic status (Greenfield 2014, Hanel and Vione 2016).

    This study, therefore, aims to answer the question of how students in environmental and sustainability related fields worldwide evaluate RVs and whether global patterns in these perceptions can be identified. To provide initial explanations for the results and to check whether a country’s wealth plays a role in the assessment of RVs, the results were correlated with selected country-specific wealth indicators. These wealth indictors were chosen because previous international studies have already shown that a country’s wealth can influence the perception of nature and environmental problems (Inglehart 1995, Franzen and Meyer 2010, Franzen and Vogl 2013, Aral and López-Sintas 2023). A recently developed method was used to simplify the analysis and the comparison of different groups within this international data set.

    METHODS

    Data collection procedure and participants

    To assess the RVs in different regions, surveys were conducted in a total of 45 countries. The selection of the 45 study countries was guided by the objective of achieving a broad geographic representation across continents and diverse socio-economic contexts. Although no strict selection criteria were applied, the aim was to include a sufficient number of countries across Asia, Africa, Europe, North and South America, and Oceania to obtain a global perspective.

    An online questionnaire was created to reach students in the individual countries. To ensure the highest possible level of data protection, the survey platform of the Goethe University Frankfurt (Germany) was used. The survey was sent by e-mail to professors, laboratory managers, and research assistants in the selected countries with the invitation to distribute it among their students. As students in the environmental fields (ecology and conservation, environmental science, biology, natural resources management, etc.) were selected as the target group, only university staff with a focus on this subject area were contacted. Researchers were selected through academic networks and institutional directories. Although institutional differences were not systematically analyzed, the study tried to include at least two universities per country, ensuring diverse academic contexts for the examination. In a text accompanying the study, the students were informed about the objectives, the voluntary nature of participation, and data protection.

    To achieve a sufficiently large data set, at least 50 researchers in each country were contacted by e-mail and asked for their support. In the e-mail, the objectives of the study, data protection, and the voluntary nature of participation were briefly explained. The questionnaire was offered in at least one official national language of each country. The translations were carried out by native speakers and checked by a second person in each case.

    The minimum sample size per country was set at 30 students. This cut-off was reached by 37 countries. Data from the eight countries that did not reach this cut-off value were excluded from the analysis. Students from disciplines not related to the environmental field and Ph.D. students were excluded before the data analysis. Only students who were studying in their country of origin were included in the analysis. Participants who indicated a different country of origin than the country where they were enrolled were excluded to minimize potential bias from international mobility. A total of 4571 (33.46% male, 64.68% female, 0.96% diverse, 0.9% no answer) questionnaires were valid according to these criteria. On average, the respondents were 22.44 (± 4.59) years old and in the fourth semester of their studies. The survey period was from September 2020 to July 2021. The sample size broken down by survey country can be found in Table 1.

    The survey was approved by the ethics committee of the science didactic institutes and Departments of the Goethe University Frankfurt am Main under approval number 15-WLSD-2104.

    Measurement instrument

    At the beginning of the questionnaire, general demographic data such as age, gender, semester of study, course of studies, university, and which country the students originally came from were recorded.

    Measuring relational values (RVs)

    Because RVs are a relatively new construct, there are only a limited number of empirical studies on this topic and no established survey instrument is yet available. Some studies assessed RVs through open-ended questionnaires or interviews (Arias-Arévalo et al. 2017, Chapman 2017). However, quantitative instruments have also been applied in recent years (Klain et al. 2017, See et al. 2020, Uehara et al. 2020, Riechers et al. 2021). In this study, the seven questions of Klain et al. (2017) were used because, on the one hand, the scale covers the construct of RVs very well and, on the other hand, its applicability has already been demonstrated in other research (Kleespies and Dierkes 2020b). The seven items and the explanation of their content validity can be found in Table 2. The seven items were to be rated on a five-point Likert scale from strongly agree to strongly disagree.

    Analysis

    It is known that the RVs are highly context dependent (Himes and Muraca 2018), and it can be expected that the different components of the RVs were assessed differently in the countries surveyed. It is, therefore, difficult to compare the data using standard procedures, such as factor analysis, as it can be assumed that the results of the factor analyses differ considerably among the countries (Append. 1). Therefore, an unsupervised learning-based clustering approach recently developed by Hahn-Klimroth et al. (2024) was used to evaluate the data collected here, which is particularly suitable for analyzing and presenting data from different groups. The aim of this analysis was to explore patterns in RVs among environmental students across different countries and how these are distributed in the individual countries. The main advantage of this method compared with a factor analysis is that no structural simplification is carried out, and all items are used jointly throughout the analysis. This means that there is no generalization of items or groups of items, as would be the case if an overall factor analysis were to be carried out with all data and the result applied to all countries.

    The method used, proposed by Hahn-Klimroth et al. (2024), is based on a well-studied unsupervised learning method, highly present in the data science community. The current contribution does not intend to present the method in detail, but we refer to the original contribution for the discussion of the statistical foundations. The method is based on cluster analysis and allows distinguishing groups with the same “typical answer” but highly different levels of group homogeneity, in contrast to factor analysis. It proceeds in three steps.

    First, missing data in the questionnaires were imputed. Here, 8-nearest neighbor imputation was chosen as an imputation technique, as this takes the correlation between different items into account, in contrast to basic imputation methods like taking the row or column mean (Troyanskaya et al. 2001). Moreover, in this step, questionnaires per country were oversampled to the number of questionnaires in the country with most questionnaires. This means that, per country, randomly chosen questionnaires were duplicated until all countries had the same number of questionnaires. This is required to obtain a meaningful clustering in the next step (Hahn-Klimroth et al. 2024). As usual in oversampling, each item’s value is perturbed with independent additive Gaussian noise with mean zero and standard deviation 0.1. This increases the stability of the clustering algorithm and reduces potential side-effects of oversampling (Min et al. 2020). Second, the set of oversampled questionnaires is clustered. The centroid of each cluster class, the “typical questionnaire in this cluster,” is called “response type.” The optimal number of cluster classes found is determined by a heuristic argument based on the so-called gap statistic (Append. 2; Hahn-Klimroth et al. 2024). Third, for each country, the proportion of questionnaires belonging to each of the six cluster classes is calculated. This quantity is a point on the sixth dimensional simplex and is called “fingerprint” (Hahn-Klimroth et al. 2024). The fingerprint is a natural description of the distribution of response types in the different countries.

    To determine whether the distribution of the individual response types in the countries is related to the wealth of a country, the percentage distribution of the response types was correlated using Spearman’s correlation with two selected prosperity indicators, the Human Development Index (HDI) and the Legatum Prosperity Index (LPI) (Fig. 1).

    The HDI (United Nations Development Programme (UNDP) 2020a) is an index that reflects life expectancy, expected years of school attendance, and the standard of living. The index can take values between 1 and 0, with 1 being the highest value (UNDP 2020b). The LPI assesses the prosperity of a nation considering 300 individual indicators in 12 overarching topics. A high score represents a high level of prosperity (Legatum Institute 2021).

    RESULTS

    Based on the data, the questionnaires can be divided into six higher-level response types. In response type 1, all seven RVs items were rated highly, without much variation. In response type 2, there was only a low level of agreement with the seven items, and here too, the variation was relatively low. In type 3, the items were rated relatively high, with the exception of RV_iden and RV_kin, which were in the medium range. The variation is slightly higher in this type. Response type 4 shows very little agreement with item RV_wild, whereas all other items have high agreement values. Type 5 shows the highest variation, with the items RV_iden, RV_kin, and RV_wild being rated lower. In the last response type, 6, agreement with the item RV_comm is particularly low, but the item RV_wild is also rated slightly lower than the other items. The variation is also comparatively high in this type (Fig. 2, Table 3). The distribution of the individual response types in the countries surveyed can be found in Table 4 and Append. 3.

    The correlations between the proportion of individual response types in the countries and the country-specific wealth indicators show values in the low to high range (Table 5).

    DISCUSSION

    The results of the analysis show that there are six different response types in the evaluation of the RVs items, which occur globally and can be distinguished from each other. The approach of determining response types and then categorizing them into higher-level fingerprints within individual countries provides a robust method for identifying response patterns across diverse groups without requiring the strict assumptions of traditional multivariate techniques such as factor analysis or principal component analysis (Yong and Pearce 2013, Hahn-Klimroth et al. 2024). A key limitation of these conventional methods is that they assume a single, consistent factor structure across all groups (Putnick and Bornstein 2016), meaning that the same underlying dimensions must explain responses in every country. However, such a consistent structure is unlikely, as RVs are highly context dependent, and their components can be perceived differently across cultures (Himes and Muraca 2018). The fingerprint approach overcomes this issue by clustering individuals based on naturally emerging response patterns rather than forcing responses into a predefined factor model. Instead of assuming that RVs are structured the same way in all countries, this method allows response types to emerge freely, reflecting cultural and contextual differences. This approach also avoids the need to statistically test for measurement invariance, which is often required in traditional factor-based analyses to ensure comparability across groups (van de Schoot et al. 2015). Another advantage of the fingerprint approach is that it focuses on identifying distinct global response types rather than averaging responses at the country level. This reduces the risk of oversimplifying complex value structures by forcing them into a uniform framework. Instead, the method highlights how different response types can exist within and across countries, offering a more flexible way to study how RVs manifest in different contexts.

    Response type 1 occurs most frequently in almost all countries. In this type, all items of the scale were rated very high, which means that the students agree with the RVs. These results demonstrate that environmental students, as future decision makers of society, see nature not only as an instrumental asset, but also as a place of connection between people and nature, as a health resource, and as an entity for whose preservation there is a moral obligation. The widespread presence of response type 1 in all countries can, therefore, be interpreted positively from the perspective of protecting ecosystems and nature, as RVs can be seen as an important motivator for the protection of nature and as a driver for environmental conservation (Knippenberg et al. 2018, Mattijssen et al. 2020, Uehara et al. 2022). In addition, RVs are an important factor in promoting positive environmental behavior (van den Born et al. 2018, Topp et al. 2022) and also provide decision makers with a possible basis for argumentation to protect parts of nature, even if an instrumental value cannot be directly demonstrated (Schulz and Martin-Ortega 2018). Different studies have already shown that RVs are widespread internationally: they have been demonstrated in Europe (Feucht et al. 2023), Asia (Uehara et al. 2020, Saito et al. 2022), Africa (Cundill et al. 2017), North America (Klain et al. 2017), South America (Guerrero-Leiva et al. 2021), and Australia (Russell and Ens 2020). The study carried out here demonstrates in one data set and in a direct comparison among countries that RVs not only exist worldwide but are also widespread. In this context, it should be noted that the uniformly high agreement with all RVs items in response type 1 may reflect a potential ceiling effect, in which high values limit the ability to distinguish among respondents. The high occurrence of response type 1 may be partly influenced by the positive framing of the survey instrument, which primarily emphasized connections with and care for nature. Although this framing was designed to align with the theoretical foundation of RVs, it may have encouraged respondents to express more relational and stewardship-oriented views than they otherwise might have in an open-ended assessment. It would be worthwhile for future research to explore whether different framing would yield different response distributions.

    The appearance of response type 1 also shows a negative medium bordering on strong correlation with the wealth indices of the countries. This means that this type occurs less frequently in wealthier countries. One possible explanation for this could be that people in these countries in particular are becoming disconnected from nature. Especially for wealthier countries, it has been documented that a lot of time is spent outside of nature (Conrad et al. 2013, Matz et al. 2014, Bassett et al. 2015). This increasing loss of human–nature interactions (Soga and Gaston 2016) might lead to a declining relationship with nature and, therefore, a decline in RVs. Interestingly, this inverse relationship contrasts with findings from wildlife value orientation research, where higher wealth, education, and urbanization have been linked to a shift toward mutualist values rather than traditionalist or instrumentalist perspectives (Manfredo et al. 2009, 2020). This seeming contradiction with Manfredo and colleagues’ findings may be explained by the conceptual differences between RVs and mutualist wildlife value orientations. Although both concepts emphasize care for nature and wildlife, mutualist orientations assume wildlife has moral equality and should be granted rights that are similar to those of humans (Manfredo et al. 2009). Relational values are broader in scope and include social and cultural dimensions in addition to the personal relationship with nature. These conceptual distinctions may help to explain why our findings differ from the patterns described by Manfredo et al. (2020).

    Whether instrumental or intrinsic value systems are more predominant in the wealthier countries cannot be determined from this data set but would require further research.

    Response type 2 stands in direct contrast to response type 1, as it represents a complete rejection of RVs. As RVs are recognized as important factors in supporting nature conservation, a wider distribution of this response type could harm environmental protection. In cases where the instrumental value of nature is unclear or difficult to quantify, RVs can provide a key argument for conservation efforts, helping to justify protective measures (de Vos et al. 2018). If relational perspectives are not considered, conservation decisions may be based solely on economic or ecological arguments, which may not always be sufficient to justify protective measures. Relational values can help secure conservation support by highlighting the cultural, social, and ethical significance of ecosystems (Arias-Arévalo et al. 2017, van Noordwijk et al. 2023). If RVs are not recognized, these arguments are no longer available, potentially reducing public and policy support for nature protection.

    However, it should be noted that this response type only occurs rarely: in more than half of the countries surveyed, this type is not found at all, and in the remaining countries, it generally only occurs in the single-digit percentage range. In addition, this response type shows no significant correlation with the wealth of the nations surveyed. This provides evidence that this response type occurs regardless of a nation’s wealth.

    In the third response type, the two items RV_iden and RV_kin were rated lower than the other RV items. These two items reflect whether nature is seen as part of one’s own personality. This part of the RVs is, therefore, very similar to the concept of connection to nature. Connection to nature generally refers to the personal relationship someone has with nature (Schultz 2002, Mayer and Frantz 2004, Tam 2013). A whole range of different measurement instruments have been developed over the last few decades to measure connection to nature, for example, the Nature Connectedness Scale by Mayer and Frantz (2004), the Inclusion of Nature in Self Scale by Schultz (2002), the Nature Relatedness Scale by Nisbet et al. (2009), or the Nature Connection Index by Richardson et al. (2019). For the two items RV_iden and RV_kin, it has also been empirically shown that they are strongly related to the Nature Connectedness Scale (Kleespies and Dierkes 2020a).

    A low value for these items, as found in response type 3, indicates that the personal connection to nature is weaker than, for example, concern for nature or the moral obligation to protect it. The personal connection to nature has repeatedly been shown to be an important motivator for the protection of nature (Mayer and Frantz 2004, Frantz et al. 2005, Kaiser et al. 2008) and has a major influence on environmental behavior (Whitburn et al. 2020, Barragan-Jason et al. 2022). Even if this group only accounts for a small percentage in most countries, it could still influence environmental protection, particularly among future decision makers whose actions will shape sustainability policies. A lower connection to nature does not necessarily mean that individuals will stop engaging in pro-environmental behaviors, as moral obligation or social responsibility can still drive conservation actions. However, connection to nature remains an important factor for fostering pro-environmental behaviors, especially for individuals who lack other personal motivations for environmental engagement. If this connection declines, it could reduce intrinsic pro-environmental behaviors, potentially weakening long-term support for sustainability efforts.

    There is also a strong positive correlation between the occurrence of this response type in a country and the selected country-specific wealth indicators. This indicates that this type, in which there is a lower connection to nature, is particularly widespread in wealthier countries. The exact factors responsible for the lower connection to nature in wealthier countries are beyond the scope of this study. One possible explanation for this could be the decline in opportunities for contact with nature (Soga and Gaston 2016) or the increase in screen time (Schultz 2002, Larson et al. 2019). This finding is consistent with other international studies that have found a decline in the connection to nature, especially in wealthier countries (Richardson et al. 2022b, Kleespies and Dierkes 2023b)

    In response type 4, the item RV_wild, which describes concern for natural places, is rated particularly low, whereas all other items show high values. In the literature, concern or care for the environment is often understood as environmental attitudes (Gifford and Sussman 2012). Environmental attitudes are considered important factors influencing environmental behavior (Minton and Rose 1997, Levine and Strube 2012), even if there are different results on the strength of this relationship (Marcinkowski and Reid 2019). As this response type is common in some countries, it is of particular importance.

    At the same time, this type shows no significant correlation with the wealth indicators. This is partly consistent with findings from the literature. Some studies have found that environmental concerns and attitudes are positively correlated with a country’s wealth, i.e., people in wealthier countries are more concerned about environmental problems (Franzen and Meyer 2010, Franzen and Vogl 2013, Aral and López-Sintas 2023). However, there are also other studies that have been unable to show such a relationship between attitudes toward the environment and a country’s wealth (Boeve-de Pauw and van Petegem 2010). It should be noted at this point that the individual item RV_wild does not cover the entire spectrum of environmental attitudes but is at best an indicator of these. Moreover, RV_wild specifically reflects concern for natural places, which is more location based and tangible compared with the broader concept of environmental attitudes. Future research should explore additional measures to better understand the link between RVs and broader environmental attitudes.

    Response type 5 can be seen as a combination of response types 3 and 4 and occurs less frequently. In this type, the three items RV_wild, RV_iden, and RV_kin are rated comparatively low. The explanations of the two previous response types can therefore also be applied to this type. However, as more parts of the RVs are rated as less important in this type, this type should be viewed more critically. The significant high correlation with the wealth indicators can probably be attributed to the connection to nature (see response type 3).

    In response type 6, the item RV_comm was rated lower than the other RV items. This item deals with cultural identity and nature as a place to connect people. As there is currently still not enough empirical research on the underlying mechanisms that shape the perception of community-building aspects of RVs (Schulz and Martin-Ortega 2018), it is difficult to determine why people see or do not see nature as a community-building element. One possible explanation could be that local communities have less contact and bonds with nature and, therefore, no longer see nature as a community unifying element. There is currently still a great need for research in order to find out exactly which factors influence the RVs core components. As there is also no significant correlation between this response type and the wealth of a country, new explanatory approaches should be specifically explored.

    IMPLICATIONS

    The six response types provide important information on how RVs are perceived by environmental students worldwide. The correlations with a nation’s wealth indicators also provide initial explanations for the distribution of these views. As RVs can influence environmental behavior and decision making, understanding their patterns could provide strategies for fostering conservation-supportive attitudes. However, different RVs types may contribute to conservation in distinct ways, depending on cultural, ecological, and policy contexts. Therefore, rather than assuming that a single response type is universally more desirable, future research should examine how various RVs translate into conservation outcomes across different settings.

    A literature review suggests that environmental education programs may help strengthen different aspects of RVs (Britto dos Santos and Gould 2018). Therefore, more environmental education could be integrated into the curriculum for environmental students to promote RVs. Uehara et al. (2020) showed that environmental education programs that take place with people from local communities, restore ecosystems, or include an active experience of a local culture in connection with nature can strengthen RVs. Active outdoor learning can also be seen as a way to increase RVs (van den Born et al. 2018). Sharing experiences with a mentor, for example, through outdoor experiences, may also be a way to improve RVs (Chan et al. 2016).

    There is a whole range of research results on what measures can increase the personal connection to nature, which would be particularly recommended for response types 3 and 5: for example, the time a person spends in nature is an important impact factor (Lengieza and Swim 2021), but also environmental education (Barrable and Booth 2020) and active experiences of nature (Richardson et al. 2022a). Various studies have shown that contact with nature and mindfulness practices are particularly effective in promoting a connection to nature (Barragan-Jason et al. 2022). There are a number of other suggestions for universities that can help to improve students’ personal relationship with nature, such as the direct integration of local nature into courses or the use of new technologies to establish a relationship with nature (Kleespies and Dierkes 2023a). Also, weakened positive environmental attitudes, as found in questionnaire types 4 and 5, might be improved by spending time in nature (DeVille et al. 2021).

    Although the fingerprint-based approach adopted here has the advantage of identifying response patterns across countries without assuming measurement invariance, it is important to note that it is not without limitations. It is essential to recognize that valuation studies, including this one, are shaped by methodological choices that influence outcomes (Jacobs et al. 2023). The survey sample consisted exclusively of university students, which may limit the generalizability of the findings and exclude perspectives from other demographic or social groups. Furthermore, the use of a standardized survey format may have resulted in the exclusion of culturally specific expressions of RVs, particularly those rooted in local traditions.

    LIMITATIONS

    Although the study was conducted with great care, there are some limitations that need to be addressed. For example, in some countries, the sample size is comparatively small, which may have limited the representation of diversity within the student population of these countries. In addition, only students in the environmental field were surveyed. Our student sample, therefore, does not fully reflect the socioeconomic composition of national populations. University students are often younger and come from more educated and economically advantaged backgrounds than the general population (Greenfield 2014, Hanel and Vione 2016), which may influence their RVs orientations. The results can, therefore, not be transferred directly to other students or the general population of a country. This is particularly relevant when interpreting correlations between the fingerprints and national economic indicators, as these relationships may reflect patterns specific to the student sub-samples rather than broader societal trends. However, as the same group was surveyed in all countries, the results remain comparable. Nevertheless, the data provide important initial indications of the distribution of RVs in a global context.

    It should also be noted that the survey was voluntary and anonymous. This may have led to a self-selection of participants: it is possible that more people filled out the questionnaire who are particularly interested in environmental issues. As this factor plays a role in all countries, the results remain comparable.

    Another limitation of this study is the gender imbalance in the sample, as women were overrepresented among the surveyed students. This is a common pattern in survey-based research, as studies have shown that women are generally more likely to participate in surveys, particularly in web-based questionnaires (Becker 2022). Although this higher participation rate is expected, it may have influenced the results, as research suggests that women tend to express stronger RVs than men (Kleespies and Dierkes 2020b). As a result, the observed distribution of RVs may skew toward higher reported agreement with RVs, potentially affecting cross-group comparisons. Although this does not invalidate the findings, it highlights the need for future research to examine whether similar RV patterns emerge in more gender-balanced populations and to further explore how gender-related participation biases may influence RVs assessments.

    In addition, it cannot be ruled out that, despite careful translations, the meaning of the questions was slightly altered, and they were, therefore, perceived differently by the study participants. Although attempts were made to address this problem by reviewing the translations, this factor cannot be completely prevented.

    Moreover, data collection took place during the COVID-19 pandemic, a period in which many people spent more time outdoors and reported a heightened appreciation of nature (Morse et al. 2020). This may have temporarily influenced respondents’ relational values, potentially leading to stronger expressions of RVs than under normal conditions.

    CONCLUSION

    The results of the study provide an initial overview of RVs among environmental students worldwide. It is positive to note that a large proportion of students in most countries belong to response type 1, which reflect strong RVs. In the future, possibilities should be examined to strengthen students’ RVs, as these can be seen as an important opportunity to improve environmental behavior. However, further research is needed to determine which specific measures are suitable to achieve this goal.

    The correlation between the wealth indicators also shows that questionnaire type 1 is less common in wealthier countries, whereas questionnaire types 3 and 5 occur more frequently. For this reason, the possible deficit in these countries in particular should be further analyzed and investigated. In this way, an important contribution can be made to the preservation of ecosystems, nature, and the environment.

    RESPONSES TO THIS ARTICLE

    Responses to this article are invited. If accepted for publication, your response will be hyperlinked to the article. To submit a response, follow this link. To read responses already accepted, follow this link.

    AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

    Conceptualization: MWK, PWD; data collection: MWK; methodology: MHK, MWK, PWD; validation, formal analysis, investigation: MWK, MHK, PWD; figures: PWD, MHK; writing – original: MWK, MHK; writing – review and editing: MWK, MHK, PWD, funding acquisition: PWD. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    We thank all study participants and the more than 300 researchers and universities that shared our questionnaires. This study was partly supported by the Opel-Zoo foundation professorship in zoo biology from the “von Opel Hessische Zoostiftung.”

    Use of Artificial Intelligence (AI) and AI-assisted Tools

    The language software DeepL was used for linguistic adaptations and edits.

    DATA AVAILABILITY

    The data and code that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author, MWK.

    LITERATURE CITED

    Alshuwaikhat, H. M., and I. Abubakar. 2008. An integrated approach to achieving campus sustainability: assessment of the current campus environmental management practices. Journal of Cleaner Production 16(16):1777-1785. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2007.12.002

    Aral, Ö. H., and J. López-Sintas. 2023. Environmental behavior patterns across clusters of European Union countries: uncovering heterogeneity in the attitude–behavior–context relationship. Journal of Cleaner Production 388:135936. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2023.135936

    Arias-Arévalo, P., B. Martín-López, and E. Gómez-Baggethun. 2017. Exploring intrinsic, instrumental, and relational values for sustainable management of social-ecological systems. Ecology and Society 22(4):43. https://doi.org/10.5751/ES-09812-220443

    Barrable, A., and D. Booth. 2020. Increasing nature connection in children: a mini review of interventions. Frontiers in Psychology 11:492. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00492

    Barragan-Jason, G., C. de Mazancourt, C. Parmesan, M. C. Singer, and M. Loreau. 2022. Human–nature connectedness as a pathway to sustainability: a global meta-analysis. Conservation Letters 15(1):e12852. https://doi.org/10.1111/conl.12852

    Bassett, D. R., D. John, S. A. Conger, E. C. Fitzhugh, and D. P. Coe. 2015. Trends in physical activity and sedentary behaviors of United States youth. Journal of Physical Activity and Health 12(8):1102-1111. https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.2014-0050

    Becker, R. 2022. Gender and survey participation an event history analysis of the gender effects of survey participation in a probability-based multi-wave panel study with a sequential mixed-mode design. Methods, Data, Analyses 16(1):3-32. https://doi.org/10.12758/mda.2021.08

    Bellou, C., V. Petreniti, and C. Skanavis. 2017. Greening the campus intentions: a study of the University of the Aegean non-academic staff. International Journal of Sustainability in Higher Education 18(4):520-532. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJSHE-05-2015-0102

    Boeve-de Pauw, J., and P. van Petegem. 2010. A cross-national perspective on youth environmental attitudes. The Environmentalist 30(2):133-144. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10669-009-9253-1

    Britto dos Santos, N., and R. K. Gould. 2018. Can relational values be developed and changed? Investigating relational values in the environmental education literature. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 35:124-131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2018.10.019

    Bullock, C. H. 2017. Nature’s values: from intrinsic to instrumental. A review of values and valuation methodologies in the context of ecosystem services and natural capital. Research Series, paper no. 10. National Economic and Social Council, Dublin, Ireland. https://www.eesc.europa.eu/ceslink/sites/default/files/document-file-uploads/research_series_paper_10_cbullock_naturesvalue.pdf.

    Chan, K. M. A., P. Balvanera, K. Benessaiah, M. Chapman, S. Díaz, E. Gómez-Baggethun, R. Gould, N. Hannahs, K. Jax, S. Klain, G. W. Luck, B. Martín-López, B. Muraca, B. Norton, K. Ott, U. Pascual, T. Satterfield, M. Tadaki, J. Taggart, and N. Turner. 2016. Opinion: why protect nature? Rethinking values and the environment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113(6):1462-1465. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1525002113

    Chan, K. M. A., R. K. Gould, and U. Pascual. 2018. Editorial overview: relational values: what are they, and what’s the fuss about? Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 35:A1-A7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2018.11.003

    Chang, R. 2015. Value pluralism. Pages 21-26 in J. D. Wright, editor. International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences, Elsevier, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.63090-8

    Chapman, M. A. 2017. Agri-‘culture’ and biodiversity: rethinking payments for ecosystem services in light of relational values. Dissertation. University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada. https://doi.org/10.14288/1.0362233

    Conrad, A., M. Seiwert, A. Hünken, D. Quarcoo, M. Schlaud, and D. Groneberg. 2013. The German environmental survey for children (GerES IV): reference values and distributions for time-location patterns of German children. International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health 216(1):25-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijheh.2012.02.004

    Cundill, G., J. C. Bezerra, A. de Vos, and N. Ntingana. 2017. Beyond benefit sharing: place attachment and the importance of access to protected areas for surrounding communities. Ecosystem Services 28:140-148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2017.03.011

    de Vos, A., C. B. Joana, and R. Dirk. 2018. Relational values about nature in protected area research. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 35:89-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2018.10.018

    DeVille, N. V., L. P. Tomasso, O. P. Stoddard, G. E. Wilt, T. H. Horton, K. L. Wolf, E. Brymer, P. H. Kahn, and P. James. 2021. Time spent in nature is associated with increased pro-environmental attitudes and behaviors. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18(14):7498. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18147498

    Díaz, S., S. Demissew, J. Carabias, C. Joly, M. Lonsdale, N. Ash, A. Larigauderie, J. R. Adhikari, S. Arico, A. Báldi, A. Bartuska, I. A. Baste, A. Bilgin, E. Brondizio, K. M. A. Chan, V. E. Figueroa, A. Duraiappah, M. Fischer, R. Hill, T. Koetz, P. Leadley, P. Lyver, G. M. Mace, B. Martin-Lopez, M. Okumura, D. Pacheco, U. Pascual, E. S. Pérez, B. Reyers, E. Roth, O. Saito, R. J. Scholes, N. Sharma, H. Tallis, R. Thaman, R. Watson, T. Yahara, Z. A. Hamid, C. Akosim, Y. Al-Hafedh, R. Allahverdiyev, E. Amankwah, S. T. Asah, Z. Asfaw, G. Bartus, L. A. Brooks, J. Caillaux, G. Dalle, D. Darnaedi, A. Driver, G. Erpul, P. Escobar-Eyzaguirre, P. Failler, A. M. M. Fouda, B. Fu, H. Gundimeda, S. Hashimoto, F. Homer, S. Lavorel, G. Lichtenstein, W. A. Mala, W. Mandivenyi, P. Matczak, C. Mbizvo, M. Mehrdadi, J. P. Metzger, J. B. Mikissa, H. Moller, H. A. Mooney, P. Mumby, H. Nagendra, C. Nesshover, A. A. Oteng-Yeboah, G. Pataki, M. Roué, J. Rubis, M. Schultz, P. Smith, R. Sumaila, K. Takeuchi, S. Thomas, M. Verma, Y. Yeo-Chang, and D. Zlatanova. 2015. The IPBES conceptual framework—connecting nature and people. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 14:1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2014.11.002

    Feucht, V., P. W. Dierkes, and M. W. Kleespies. 2023. The different values of nature: a comparison between university students’ perceptions of nature’s instrumental, intrinsic and relational values. Sustainability Science 18(5):2391-2403. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-023-01371-8

    Frantz, C., F. S. Mayer, C. Norton, and M. Rock. 2005. There is no “I” in nature: the influence of self-awareness on connectedness to nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology 25(4):427-436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.10.002

    Franzen, A., and R. Meyer. 2010. Environmental attitudes in cross-national perspective: a multilevel analysis of the ISSP 1993 and 2000. European Sociological Review 26(2):219-234. https://doi.org/10.1093/esr/jcp018

    Franzen, A., and D. Vogl. 2013. Two decades of measuring environmental attitudes: a comparative analysis of 33 countries. Global Environmental Change 23(5):1001-1008. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.03.009

    Gifford, R., and R. Sussman. 2012. Environmental attitudes. Pages 65-80 in S. D. Clayton, editor. The Oxford handbook of environmental and conservation psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199733026.013.0004

    Greenfield, P. M. 2014. Sociodemographic differences within countries produce variable cultural values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 45(1):37-41. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022113513402

    Guerrero-Leiva, N., C. Cerda, and I. Bidegain. 2021. Residential sidewalk gardens and biological conservation in the cities: motivations and preferences that guide the floristic composition of a little-explored space. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening 63:127227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ufug.2021.127227

    Hahn-Klimroth, M., P. W. Dierkes, and M. W. Kleespies. 2024. An unsupervised learning approach to evaluate questionnaire data—what one can learn from violations of measurement invariance. Data Science Journal 23(13):1-17. https://doi.org/10.5334/dsj-2024-013

    Hanel, P. H. P., and K. C. Vione. 2016. Do student samples provide an accurate estimate of the general public? PLoS ONE 11(12):e0168354. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168354

    Himes, A., and B. Muraca. 2018. Relational values: the key to pluralistic valuation of ecosystem services. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 35:1-7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2018.09.005

    Himes, A., B. Muraca, C. B. Anderson, S. Athayde, T. Beery, M. Cantú-Fernández, D. González-Jimínez, R. K. Gould, A. P. Hejnowicz, J. Kenter, D. Lenzi, R. Murali, U. Pascual, C. Raymond, A. Ring, K. Russo, A. Samakov, S. Stålhammar, H. Thorén, and E. Zent. 2024. Why nature matters: a systematic review of intrinsic, instrumental, and relational values. BioScience 74(1):25-43. https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biad109

    Inglehart, R. 1995. Public support for environmental protection: objective problems and subjective values in 43 societies. PS: Political Science and Politics 28(1):57-72. https://doi.org/10.2307/420583

    Intergovernmental Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services (IPBES). 2018. Contrasting approaches to values and valuation: value monism vs. value pluralism in policy. Intergovernmental Science-Policy Platform on Biodiversity and Ecosystem Services, Bonn, Germany. https://www.ipbes.net/contrasting-approaches-values-valuation

    Jacobs, S., E. Kelemen, P. O’Farrell, A. Martin, M. Schaafsma, N. Dendoncker, R. Pandit, T. H. Mwampamba, I. Palomo, A. J. Castro, M. A. Huambachano, A. Filyushkina, and H. Gunimeda. 2023. The pitfalls of plural valuation. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 64:101345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2023.101345

    Jacobs, S., N. Zafra-Calvo, D. Gonzalez-Jimenez, L. Guibrunet, K. Benessaiah, A. Berghöfer, J. Chaves-Chaparro, S. Díaz, E. Gomez-Baggethun, S. Lele, B. Martín-López, V. A. Masterson, J. Merçon, H. Moersberger, B. Muraca, A. Norström, P. O’Farrell, J. C. Ordonez, A.-H. Prieur-Richard, A. Rincón-Ruiz, N. Sitas, S. M. Subramanian, W. Tadesse, M. van Noordwijk, U. Pascual, and P. Balvanera. 2020. Use your power for good: plural valuation of nature—the Oaxaca statement. Global Sustainability 3:e8. https://doi.org/10.1017/sus.2020.2

    Justus, J., M. Colyvan, H. Regan, and L. Maguire. 2009. Buying into conservation: intrinsic versus instrumental value. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 24(4):187-191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2008.11.011

    Kaiser, F. G., N. Roczen, and F. X. Bogner. 2008. Competence formation in environmental education: advancing ecology-specific rather than general abilities. Umweltpsychologie 12(2):56-70.

    Kioupi, V., and N. Voulvoulis. 2020. Sustainable development goals (SDGs): assessing the contribution of higher education programmes. Sustainability 12(17):6701. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12176701

    Klain, S. C., P. Olmsted, K. M. A. Chan, and T. Satterfield. 2017. Relational values resonate broadly and differently than intrinsic or instrumental values, or the new ecological paradigm. PLoS ONE 12(8):e0183962. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183962

    Kleespies, M. W., and P. W. Dierkes. 2020a. Exploring the construct of relational values: an empirical approach. Frontiers in Psychology 11:209. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00209

    Kleespies, M. W., and P. W. Dierkes. 2020b. Impact of biological education and gender on students’ connection to nature and relational values. PLoS ONE 15(11):e0242004. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0242004

    Kleespies, M. W., and P. W. Dierkes. 2023a. Connection to nature for sustainable development at universities—what should be done? Frontiers in Sustainability 4:1249328. https://doi.org/10.3389/frsus.2023.1249328

    Kleespies, M. W., and P. W. Dierkes. 2023b. Connection to nature of university students in the environmental field—an empirical study in 41 countries. Biological Conservation 283:110093. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2023.110093

    Knippenberg, L., W. T. de Groot, R. J. G. van den Born, P. Knights, and B. Muraca. 2018. Relational value, partnership, eudaimonia: a review. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 35:39-45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2018.10.022

    Larson, L. R., R. Szczytko, E. P. Bowers, L. E. Stephens, K. T. Stevenson, and M. F. Floyd. 2019. Outdoor time, screen time, and connection to nature: troubling trends among rural youth? Environment and Behavior 51(8):966-991. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916518806686

    Legatum Institute. 2021. The Legatum prosperity index™: a tool for transformation. Legatum Institute Foundation, London, UK. https://docs.prosperity.com/3616/3544/3967/The_2021_Legatum_Prosperity_Index_Overview_-_Web.pdf.

    Lengieza, M. L., and J. K. Swim. 2021. The paths to connectedness: a review of the antecedents of connectedness to nature. Frontiers in Psychology 12:763231. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.763231

    Levine, D. S., and M. J. Strube. 2012. Environmental attitudes, knowledge, intentions and behaviors among college students. The Journal of Social Psychology 152(3):308-326. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2011.604363

    Lozano, R. 2006. Incorporation and institutionalization of SD into universities: breaking through barriers to change. Journal of Cleaner Production 14(9-11):787-796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2005.12.010

    Manfredo, M. J., T. L. Teel, A. W. Don Carlos, L. Sullivan, A. D. Bright, A. M. Dietsch, J. Bruskotter, and D. Fulton. 2020. The changing sociocultural context of wildlife conservation. Conservation Biology 34(6):1549-1559. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13493

    Manfredo, M. J., T. L. Teel, and K. L. Henry. 2009. Linking society and environment: a multilevel model of shifting wildlife value orientations in the western United States. Social Science Quarterly 90(2):407-427. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2009.00624.x

    Marcinkowski, T., and A. Reid. 2019. Reviews of research on the attitude–behavior relationship and their implications for future environmental education research. Environmental Education Research 25(4):459-471. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504622.2019.1634237

    Martin, A., P. Balvanera, C. Raymond, E. Gómez-Baggethun, U. Eser, R. Gould, L. Guibrunet, Z. V. Harmáčková, A. Horcea-Milcu, A.-K. Koessler, R. Kumar, D. Lenzi, J. Merçon, A. Nthenge, P. O’Farrell, U. Pascual, J. Rode, Y. Yoshida, and N. Zafra-Calvo. 2024. Sustainability-aligned values: exploring the concept, evidence, and practice. Ecology and Society 29(4):18. https://doi.org/10.5751/ES-15498-290418

    Mattijssen, T. J., W. Ganzevoort, R. J. van den Born, B. J. Arts, B. C. Breman, A. E. Buijs, R. I. van Dam, B. H. Elands, W. T. de Groot, and L. W. Knippenberg. 2020. Relational values of nature: leverage points for nature policy in Europe. Ecosystems and People 16(1):402-410. https://doi.org/10.1080/26395916.2020.1848926

    Matz, C. J., D. M. Stieb, K. Davis, M. Egyed, A. Rose, B. Chou, and O. Brion. 2014. Effects of age, season, gender and urban–rural status on time-activity: Canadian human activity pattern survey 2 (CHAPS 2). International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 11(2):2108-2124. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph110202108

    Mayer, F., and C. M. Frantz. 2004. The connectedness to nature scale: a measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology 24(4):503-515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001

    McCauley, D. J. 2006. Selling out on nature. Nature 443(7107):27-28. https://doi.org/10.1038/443027a

    Min, J., R. T. McCoy, D. Das, E. Pitler, and T. Linzen. 2020. Syntactic data augmentation increases robustness to inference heuristics. Pages 2339-2352 in D. Jurafsky, J. Chai, N. Schluter, and J. Tetreault, editors. Proceedings of the 58th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics. Association for Computational Linguistics, Stroudsburg, Pennsylvania, USA. https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/2020.acl-main.212

    Minton, A. P., and R. L. Rose. 1997. The effects of environmental concern on environmentally friendly consumer behavior: an exploratory study. Journal of Business Research 40(1):37-48. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0148-2963(96)00209-3

    Morse, J. W., T. M. Gladkikh, D. M. Hackenburg, and R. K. Gould. 2020. COVID-19 and human–nature relationships: Vermonters’ activities in nature and associated nonmaterial values during the pandemic. PLoS ONE 15(12):e0243697. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243697

    Nisbet, E. K., J. M. Zelenski, and S. A. Murphy. 2009. Nature relatedness scale. Environment and Behavior 41(5):715-740. https://doi.org/10.1037/t05985-000

    Pascual, U., P. Balvanera, C. B. Anderson, R. Chaplin-Kramer, M. Christie, D. González-Jimínez, A. Martin, C. M. Raymond, M. Termansen, A. Vatn, S. Athayde, B. Baptiste, D. N. Barton, S. Jacobs, E. Kelemen, R. Kumar, E. Lazos, T. H. Mwampamba, B. Nakangu, P. O’Farrell, S. M. Subramanian, M. van Noordwijk, S. Ahn, S. Amaruzaman, A. M. Amin, P. Arias-Arévalo, G. Arroyo-Robles, M. Cantú-Fernández, A. J. Castro, V. Contreras, A. de Vos, N. Dendoncker, S. Engel, U. Eser, D. P. Faith, A. Filyushkina, H. Ghazi, E. Gómez-Baggethun, R. K. Gould, L. Guibrunet, H. Gundimeda, T. Hahn, Z. V. Harmáčková, M. Hernández-Blanco, A.-I. Horcea-Milcu, M. Huambachano, N. L. H. Wicher, C. İ. Aydın, M. Islar, A.-K. Koessler, J. O. Kenter, M. Kosmus, H. Lee, B. Leimona, S. Lele, D. Lenzi, B. Lliso, L. M. Mannetti, J. Merçon, A. S. Monroy-Sais, N. Mukherjee, B. Muraca, R. Muradian, R. Murali, S. H. Nelson, G. R. Nemogná-Soto, J. Ngouhouo-Poufoun, A. Niamir, E. Nuesiri, T. O. Nyumba, B. Özkaynak, I. Palomo, R. Pandit, A. Pawłowska-Mainville, L. Porter-Bolland, M. Quaas, J. Rode, R. Rozzi, S. Sachdeva, A. Samakov, M. Schaafsma, N. Sitas, P. Ungar, E. Yiu, Y. Yoshida, and E. Zent. 2023. Diverse values of nature for sustainability. Nature 620(7975):813-823. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-023-06406-9

    Pascual, U., P. Balvanera, S. Díaz, G. Pataki, E. Roth, M. Stenseke, R. T. Watson, E. Başak Dessane, M. Islar, E. Kelemen, V. Maris, M. Quaas, S. M. Subramanian, H. Wittmer, A. Adlan, S. Ahn, Y. S. Al-Hafedh, E. Amankwah, S. T. Asah, P. Berry, A. Bilgin, S. J. Breslow, C. Bullock, D. Cáceres, H. Daly-Hassen, E. Figueroa, C. D. Golden, E. Gómez-Baggethun, D. González-Jimínez, J. Houdet, H. Keune, R. Kumar, K. Ma, P. H. May, A. Mead, P. O’Farrell, R. Pandit, W. Pengue, R. Pichis-Madruga, F. Popa, S. Preston, D. Pacheco-Balanza, H. Saarikoski, B. B. Strassburg, M. van den Belt, M. Verma, F. Wickson, and N. Yagi. 2017. Valuing nature’s contributions to people: the IPBES approach. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 26-27:7-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2016.12.006

    Pratson, D. F., N. Adams, and R. K. Gould. 2023. Relational values of nature in empirical research: a systematic review. People and Nature 5(5):1464-1479. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10512

    Putnick, D. L., and M. H. Bornstein. 2016. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Developmental Review 41:71-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004

    Reyers, B., S. Polasky, H. Tallis, H. A. Mooney, and A. Larigauderie. 2012. Finding common ground for biodiversity and ecosystem services. BioScience 62(5):503-507. https://doi.org/10.1525/bio.2012.62.5.12

    Richardson, M., I. Hamlin, C. W. Butler, R. Thomas, and A. Hunt. 2022a. Actively noticing nature (not just time in nature) helps promote nature connectedness. Ecopsychology 14(1):8-16. https://doi.org/10.1089/eco.2021.0023

    Richardson, M., I. Hamlin, L. R. Elliott, and M. P. White. 2022b. Country-level factors in a failing relationship with nature: nature connectedness as a key metric for a sustainable future. Ambio 51(11):2201-2213. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13280-022-01744-w

    Richardson, M., A. Hunt, J. Hinds, R. Bragg, D. Fido, D. Petronzi, L. Barbett, T. Clitherow, and M. White. 2019. A measure of nature connectedness for children and adults: validation, performance, and insights. Sustainability 11(12):3250. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11123250

    Riechers, M., Á. Balázsi, J.-O. Engler, G. Shumi, and J. Fischer. 2021. Understanding relational values in cultural landscapes in Romania and Germany. People and Nature 3(5):1036-1046. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10246

    Riechers, M., J. Pearson, N. Diaz-Cruz, S. Ortiz-Przychodzka, and E. Topp. 2025. Interplays between relational and instrumental values: insights from research experiences on human–nature relations. Sustainability Science 20(1):287-298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-024-01559-6

    Russell, S., and E. Ens. 2020. Connection as country: relational values of billabongs in indigenous northern Australia. Ecosystem Services 45:101169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2020.101169

    Saito, T., S. Hashimoto, and M. Basu. 2022. Measuring relational values: do people in Greater Tokyo appreciate place-based nature and general nature differently? Sustainability Science 17(3):837-848. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-020-00898-4

    Sandler, R. 2012. Intrinsic value, ecology, and conservation. Nature Education Knowledge 3(10):4. https://www.nature.com/scitable/knowledge/library/intrinsic-value-ecology-and-conservation-25815400/.

    Schultz, P. W. 2002. Inclusion with nature: the psychology of human–nature relations. Pages 61-78 in P. Schmuck, and W. P. Schultz, editors. Psychology of sustainable development. Springer, Boston, Massachusetts, USA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0995-0_4

    Schulz, C., and J. Martin-Ortega. 2018. Quantifying relational values—why not? Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 35:15-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2018.10.015

    See, S. C., S. F. E. A. Shaikh, W. Jaung, and L. R. Carrasco. 2020. Are relational values different in practice to instrumental values? Ecosystem Services 44:101132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2020.101132

    Soga, M., and K. J. Gaston. 2016. Extinction of experience: the loss of human–nature interactions. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 14(2):94-101. https://doi.org/10.1002/fee.1225

    Soulé, M. 1985. What is conservation biology? BioScience 35(11):727-734. https://doi.org/10.2307/1310054

    Soulé, M. 2014. The “new conservation”. Pages 66-80 in G. Wuerthner, E. Crist, and T. Butler, editors. Keeping the wild: against the domestication of Earth. Island Press, Washington, D.C., USA. https://doi.org/10.5822/978-1-61091-559-5_7

    Tallis, H., and J. Lubchenco. 2014. Working together: a call for inclusive conservation. Nature 515(7525):27-28. https://doi.org/10.1038/515027a

    Tam, K.-P. 2013. Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: similarities and differences. Journal of Environmental Psychology 34:64-78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.01.004

    Topp, E. N., J. Loos, and B. Martín-López. 2022. Decision-making for nature’s contributions to people in the Cape Floristic region: the role of values, rules and knowledge. Sustainability Science 17(3):739-760. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-020-00896-6

    Troyanskaya, O., M. Cantor, G. Sherlock, P. Brown, T. Hastie, R. Tibshirani, D. Botstein, and R. B. Altman. 2001. Missing value estimation methods for DNA microarrays. Bioinformatics 17(6):520-525. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/17.6.520

    Uehara, T., R. Sakurai, and T. Hidaka. 2022. The importance of relational values in gaining people’s support and promoting their involvement in social-ecological system management: a comparative analysis. Frontiers in Marine Science 9:1001180. https://doi.org/10.3389/fmars.2022.1001180

    Uehara, T., R. Sakurai, and T. Tsuge. 2020. Cultivating relational values and sustaining socio-ecological production landscapes through ocean literacy: a study on Satoumi. Environment, Development and Sustainability 22(2):1599-1616. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10668-018-0226-8

    United Nations Development Programme (UNDP). 2020a. Human development report 2020. The next frontier: human development and the Anthropocene. UNDP, New York, New York, USA. https://hdr.undp.org/content/human-development-report-2020

    United Nations Development Programme (UNDP). 2020b. Technical notes: calculating the human development indices—graphical presentation. UNDP, New York, New York, USA. https://hdr.undp.org/sites/default/files/2021-22_HDR/hdr2021-22_technical_notes.pdf

    van de Schoot, R., P. Schmidt, A. de Beuckelaer, K. Lek, and M. Zondervan-Zwijnenburg. 2015. Editorial: measurement invariance. Frontiers in Psychology 6:1064. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01064

    van den Born, R. J., B. Arts, J. Admiraal, A. Beringer, P. Knights, E. Molinario, K. P. Horvat, C. Porras-Gomez, A. Smrekar, N. Soethe, J. L. Vivero-Pol, W. Ganzevoort, M. Bonaiuto, L. Knippenberg, and W. T. de Groot. 2018. The missing pillar: eudemonic values in the justification of nature conservation. Journal of Environmental Planning and Management 61(5-6):841-856. https://doi.org/10.1080/09640568.2017.1342612

    van Noordwijk, M., G. B. Villamor, G. J. Hofstede, and E. N. Speelman. 2023. Relational versus instrumental perspectives on values of nature and resource management decisions. Current Opinion in Environmental Sustainability 65:101374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cosust.2023.101374

    Vicente-Molina, M. A., A. Fernández-Sáinz, and J. Izagirre-Olaizola. 2013. Environmental knowledge and other variables affecting pro-environmental behaviour: comparison of university students from emerging and advanced countries. Journal of Cleaner Production 61:130-138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2013.05.015

    Whitburn, J., W. Linklater, and W. Abrahamse. 2020. Meta-analysis of human connection to nature and proenvironmental behavior. Conservation Biology 34(1):180-193. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13381

    Yong, A. G., and S. Pearce. 2013. A beginner’s guide to factor analysis: focusing on exploratory factor analysis. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology 9(2):79-94. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.09.2.p079

    Corresponding author:
    Matthias Kleespies
    kleespies@em.uni-frankfurt.de
    Appendix 1
    Appendix 2
    Appendix 3
    Fig. 1
    Fig. 1. Graphical abstract.

    Fig. 1. Graphical abstract.

    Fig. 1
    Fig. 2
    Fig. 2. Representation of the average response behavior in the six response types. Five indicates strong agreement with an item, 1 disagreement. The black line represents the mean value, the gray area the standard deviation.

    Fig. 2. Representation of the average response behavior in the six response types. Five indicates strong agreement with an item, 1 disagreement. The black line represents the mean value, the gray area the standard deviation.

    Fig. 2
    Table 1
    Table 1. Official names of the countries surveyed with sample size (<em>n</em>), gender distribution (m, f, d, N/A), semester of study of respondents (1–9), average age with standard deviation (Age ± SD) and number of universities (NU) surveyed.

    Table 1. Official names of the countries surveyed with sample size (n), gender distribution (m, f, d, N/A), semester of study of respondents (1–9), average age with standard deviation (Age ± SD) and number of universities (NU) surveyed.

    Official country name n Gender Semester Age ± SD NU
    m f d N/A 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9+
    Canada 190 50 131 6 3 7 41 12 38 17 26 11 20 12 22.09 ± 3.83 6
    Commonwealth of Australia 103 38 61 3 1 1 6 6 30 12 33 5 4 6 22.79 ± 6.76 2
    Commonwealth of Puerto Rico 57 10 45 0 0 13 2 12 3 6 4 6 1 8 20.44 ± 2.76 2
    Dominican Republic 112 36 76 0 0 3 0 5 11 13 8 13 20 39 23.97 ± 6.00 4
    Federal Republic of Germany 457 122 322 10 3 26 280 23 32 29 25 11 15 9 21.46 ± 2.88 2
    Federal Republic of Nigeria 85 47 38 0 0 4 23 15 5 1 1 2 6 26 30.63 ± 6.54 3
    Federative Republic of Brazil 96 33 61 1 1 5 7 22 11 4 9 2 9 21 23.32 ± 4.80 4
    French Republic 115 51 63 0 1 16 1 3 0 26 5 19 1 42 22.30 ± 2.84 2
    Islamic Republic of Pakistan 101 24 77 0 0 21 3 30 7 6 10 9 5 8 24.25 ± 4.33 6
    Japan 59 26 32 0 1 17 11 9 10 1 1 2 2 6 23.93 ± 4.64 5
    Kingdom of Morocco 46 11 32 0 3 0 1 3 0 28 2 2 2 2 23.08 ± 3.74 2
    Kingdom of Saudi Arabia 120 118 2 0 0 46 59 0 0 2 3 1 0 0 22.56 ± 1.67 2
    Kingdom of Spain 295 101 188 3 3 190 8 19 3 24 2 16 9 17 21.00 ± 4.50 4
    Kingdom of Sweden 49 12 36 1 0 1 11 4 12 0 4 1 1 13 25.19 ± 3.16 1
    Kingdom of Thailand 66 15 51 0 0 13 23 11 17 1 0 0 0 1 22.00 ± 4.16 4
    People's Republic of China 105 36 68 1 0 5 38 7 22 1 15 0 11 5 20.97 ± 2.54 2
    Portuguese Republic 204 91 110 1 2 56 35 16 14 21 24 10 4 21 22.92 ± 5.39 3
    Republic of China 184 101 82 0 1 16 33 18 36 6 38 5 19 9 20.98 ± 2.45 4
    Republic of Colombia 120 49 69 0 2 5 7 20 10 16 18 18 12 12 24.58 ± 7.61 7
    Republic of Costa Rica 30 11 19 0 0 0 6 3 4 1 1 1 6 6 22.89 ± 4.96 2
    Republic of Ecuador 29 12 17 0 0 1 2 4 1 6 0 4 2 9 21.77 ± 3.47 2
    Republic of India 57 20 36 0 1 16 0 23 6 1 0 0 2 5 23.43 ± 4.95 4
    Republic of Ireland 74 26 46 2 0 57 2 4 1 6 0 3 0 1 20.47 ± 5.48 1
    Republic of Kenya 61 36 23 0 2 1 19 2 12 0 8 2 10 6 24.83 ± 5.64 2
    Republic of Korea 48 20 26 2 0 6 1 8 2 4 0 21 0 6 23.54 ± 2.52 3
    Republic of Panama 29 8 20 0 1 0 2 0 0 1 0 2 3 20 23.96 ± 2.28 1
    Republic of Peru 122 54 65 0 3 1 53 15 8 3 13 8 2 16 22.59 ± 6.46 5
    Republic of Poland 503 125 372 4 2 140 16 108 10 98 11 54 8 52 21.60 ± 2.55 7
    Republic of Singapore 127 49 74 1 3 43 4 27 8 16 3 17 1 8 22.51 ± 3.42 2
    Republic of South Africa 30 10 20 0 0 0 9 1 3 0 4 1 1 10 26.04 ± 4.68 4
    Republic of the Philippines 264 106 157 0 1 94 76 13 17 11 20 4 3 6 21.38 ± 3.45 7
    Russian Federation 104 26 74 1 3 1 22 3 18 0 21 0 25 12 21.43 ± 3.98 3
    Slovak Republic 131 33 96 1 1 17 29 12 39 5 8 2 5 10 22.07 ± 2.87 8
    United Arab Emirates 60 60 0 0 0 33 1 0 3 2 9 2 4 5 19.59 ± 1.50 1
    United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland 97 34 58 5 0 50 4 17 3 11 0 7 0 3 21.82 ± 6.10 3
    United Mexican States 159 72 85 0 2 12 9 3 21 29 16 28 8 31 24.38 ± 4.38 7
    United States of America 81 31 48 2 0 21 1 10 6 12 1 11 6 11 23.84 ± 5.77 6
    Table 2
    Table 2. Overview of the RV items used in this study, adapted from Klain et al. (2017). In the third column, a content-related reference to the RVs concept is provided, and the individual items are assigned to the RVs categories from Chan et al. (2016) Fig. 1.

    Table 2. Overview of the RV items used in this study, adapted from Klain et al. (2017). In the third column, a content-related reference to the RVs concept is provided, and the individual items are assigned to the RVs categories from Chan et al. (2016) Fig. 1.

    Abbreviation Wording of the item Contextual justification of RVs Items (for content validity)
    RV_comm There are landscapes that say something about who we are as a community, a people. Nature (or landscape) as an element that connects people or human communities (cultural identity)
    RV_health My health or the health of my family is related one way or another to the natural environment. Nature as an element that creates health and well-being, for oneself and one’s own family; nature as a unifying element (social cohesion)
    RV_iden I have strong feelings about nature (including all plants, animals, the land, etc.) these views are part of who I am and how I live my life. Personal connection to nature; nature as part of your own identity (individual identity)
    RV_kin Plants and animals, as part of the interdependent web of life, are like “kin” or family to me, so how we treat them matters. Personal connection to nature; moral obligation to treat nature well (stewardship principle; moral responsibility)
    RV_resp How we manage the land, both for plants and animals and for future people, reflects my sense of responsibility to and so stewardship of the land. Protection and care for the ecosystem, also in relation to future generations (social responsibility; stewardship)
    RV_wild I often think of some wild places whose fate I care about and strive to protect, even though I may never see them myself. Personal moral obligation to protect nature (moral responsiblity to non-humans); Caring for the land fulfills and enriches the individual (Stewardship eudaimonic)
    RV_other Humans have a responsibility to account for our own impacts to the environment because they can harm other people. Protecting nature to protect other people (social responsebility)
    It must be noted that the individual questions cannot be completely assigned to a single RVs category, but the questions are interconnected and may align with multiple aspects of the RVs concept.
    Table 3
    Table 3. Average rating of the seven RVs items in the six response types and variation within the response types. The variation is the standard deviation of the respective response type.

    Table 3. Average rating of the seven RVs items in the six response types and variation within the response types. The variation is the standard deviation of the respective response type.

    Response type RV_comm RV_health RV_iden RV_kin RV_resp RV_wild RV_other Variation
    1 4.66 ± 0.50 4.74 ± 0.42 4.71 ± 0.51 4.66 ± 0.51 4.79 ± 0.30 4.77 ± 0.56 4.86 ± 0.13 0.33
    2 1.63 ± 0.32 1.46 ± 0.49 1.49 ± 0.50 1.21 ± 0.55 1.13 ± 0.23 1.52 ± 0.55 1.17 ± 0.14 0.44
    3 4.25 ± 0.55 3.97 ± 0.49 3.48 ± 0.59 3.38 ± 0.59 4.19 ± 0.47 4.13 ± 0.64 4.42 ± 0.14 1.09
    4 4.69 ± 0.53 4.70 ± 0.46 4.55 ± 0.53 4.63 ± 0.56 4.74 ± 0.44 2.76 ± 0.87 4.77 ± 0.15 0.83
    5 3.76 ± 0.65 4.02 ± 0.50 2.82 ± 0.91 2.81 ± 0.93 3.54 ± 0.49 2.16 ± 0.98 4.42 ± 0.18 1.22
    6 2.32 ± 0.68 4.39 ± 0.46 4.16 ± 0.56 4.43 ± 0.53 4.61 ± 0.45 3.81 ± 0.82 4.81 ± 0.14 1.09
    Table 4
    Table 4. Fingerprints of the individual countries as a percentage (distribution of the individual response types in the countries) rounded to 2 decimal points.

    Table 4. Fingerprints of the individual countries as a percentage (distribution of the individual response types in the countries) rounded to 2 decimal points.

    1 2 3 4 5 6
    Canada 58.05% 1.79% 16.90% 7.36% 6.36% 9.54%
    Commonwealth of Australia 67.99% 0.00% 6.76% 9.74% 8.75% 6.76%
    Commonwealth of Puerto Rico 67.59% 1.59% 8.75% 9.54% 3.98% 8.55%
    Dominican Republic 66.40% 4.18% 13.92% 11.33% 1.59% 2.58%
    Federal Republic of Germany 28.83% 2.39% 23.86% 5.57% 27.63% 11.73%
    Federal Republic of Nigeria 56.66% 0.00% 13.12% 14.31% 5.17% 10.74%
    Federative Republic of Brazil 72.56% 0.00% 13.52% 4.18% 6.36% 3.38%
    French Republic 42.35% 0.00% 30.42% 16.30% 5.57% 5.37%
    Islamic Republic of Pakistan 61.83% 0.00% 10.54% 8.95% 5.57% 13.12%
    Japan 26.04% 0.00% 19.68% 10.34% 30.42% 13.52%
    Kingdom of Morocco 58.05% 5.77% 3.78% 6.96% 5.57% 19.88%
    Kingdom of Saudi Arabia 46.92% 1.19% 21.27% 7.95% 6.76% 15.90%
    Kingdom of Spain 38.77% 4.18% 27.24% 10.14% 15.90% 3.78%
    Kingdom of Sweden 49.30% 0.00% 20.68% 2.58% 9.94% 17.50%
    Kingdom of Thailand 57.26% 0.00% 14.91% 6.56% 6.56% 14.71%
    People’s Republic of China 56.06% 0.00% 12.33% 17.10% 9.74% 4.77%
    Portuguese Republic 46.72% 0.00% 27.63% 10.93% 4.57% 10.14%
    Republic of China 50.50% 0.99% 19.48% 12.13% 13.32% 3.58%
    Republic of Colombia 68.99% 5.17% 13.32% 8.95% 1.99% 1.59%
    Republic of Costa Rica 68.19% 0.00% 9.94% 9.34% 6.76% 5.77%
    Republic of Ecuador 69.58% 11.93% 6.36% 8.95% 0.00% 3.18%
    Republic of India 65.81% 0.00% 9.34% 10.54% 5.77% 8.55%
    Republic of Ireland 62.23% 0.00% 17.10% 7.16% 7.36% 6.16%
    Republic of Kenya 78.73% 0.00% 9.15% 7.36% 0.00% 4.77%
    Republic of Korea 50.89% 0.00% 30.22% 4.57% 4.97% 9.34%
    Republic of Panama 62.03% 17.10% 12.72% 4.57% 0.00% 3.58%
    Republic of Peru 45.53% 6.16% 20.68% 15.51% 8.75% 3.38%
    Republic of Poland 32.60% 0.60% 19.09% 9.54% 15.31% 22.86%
    Republic of Singapore 37.57% 0.00% 16.90% 10.93% 19.09% 15.51%
    Republic of South Africa 61.83% 0.00% 13.52% 8.95% 7.55% 8.15%
    Republic of the Philippines 71.17% 0.00% 9.94% 11.33% 4.18% 3.38%
    Russian Federation 34.00% 1.59% 17.10% 10.14% 18.29% 18.89%
    Slovak Republic 44.33% 0.60% 16.90% 19.88% 8.75% 9.54%
    United Arab Emirates 67.20% 0.00% 15.71% 7.16% 0.00% 9.94%
    United Kingdom of Great Britain and Northern Ireland 54.27% 0.00% 25.65% 7.55% 5.77% 6.76%
    United Mexican States 66.00% 8.35% 9.15% 10.93% 4.77% 0.80%
    United States of America 51.89% 1.39% 14.31% 11.73% 14.91% 5.77%
    Overall average 55.26% 2.03% 16.00% 9.65% 8.32% 8.74%
    Table 5
    Table 5. Correlation between the proportion of individual response types in the countries and the two wealth indicators with corresponding <em>p</em> values. Significant correlations (<em>p</em> < 0.05) are printed in bold.

    Table 5. Correlation between the proportion of individual response types in the countries and the two wealth indicators with corresponding p values. Significant correlations (p < 0.05) are printed in bold.

    Response type Human Development Index (2020) Legatum Prosperity Index 2021
    r p r p
    1 - .483 .001 - .472 .004
    2 - .111 .525 - .096 .582
    3 .627 < .001 .603 < .001
    4 - .136 .436 - .122 .486
    5 .550 < .001 .507 .002
    6 .259 .134 .171 .325
    Click and hold to drag window
    ×
    Download PDF Download icon Download Citation Download icon Submit a Response Arrow-Forward icon
    Share
    • Twitter logo
    • LinkedIn logo
    • Facebook logo
    • Email Icon
    • Link Icon

    Keywords

    Click on a keyword to view more articles on that topic.

    decision makers; environmental students; international survey; relational values; RVs; unsupervised learning; Ward’s clustering algorithm

    Submit a response to this article

    Learn More
    See Issue Table of Contents

    Subscribe for updates

    * indicates required
    • Submit an Article
    • Submission Guidelines
    • Current Issue
    • Journal Policies
    • Find Back Issues
    • Open Access Policy
    • Find Features
    • Contact

    Resilience Alliance is a registered 501 (c)(3) non-profit organization

    Permissions and Copyright Information

    Online and Open Access since 1997

    Ecology and Society is now licensing all its articles under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License

    Ecology and Society ISSN: 1708-3087